Skip to main content

Effect of sonification types in upper-limb movement: a quantitative and qualitative study in hemiparetic and healthy participants

Abstract

Background

Movement sonification, the use of real-time auditory feedback linked to movement parameters, have been proposed to support rehabilitation. Nevertheless, if promising results have been reported, the effect of the type of sound used has not been studied systematically. The aim of this study was to investigate in a single session the effect of different types of sonification both quantitatively and qualitatively on patients with acquired brain lesions and healthy participants.

Methods

An experimental setup enabling arm sonification was developed using three different categories of sonification (direct sound modulation, musical interaction, and soundscape). Simple moving forward movements performed while sliding on a table with both arms were investigated with all participants. Quantitative analysis on the movement timing were performed considering various parameters (sound condition, affected arm and dominance, sonification categories). Qualitative analysis of semi-structured interviews were also conducted, as well as neuropsychological evaluation of music perception.

Results

For both the patient and healthy groups (15 participants each), average duration for performing the arm movement is significantly longer with sonification compared to the no-sound condition (p < 0.001). Qualitative analysis of semi-structured interviews revealed different aspects of motivational and affective aspects of sonification. Most participants of both groups preferred to complete the task with sound (29 of 30 participants), and described the experience as playful (22 of 30 participants). More precisely, the soundscape (nature sounds) was the most constantly preferred (selected first by 14 of 30 participants).

Conclusion

Overall, our results confirm that the sonification has an effect on the temporal execution of the movement during a single-session. Globally, sonification is welcomed by the participants, and we found convergent and differentiated appreciations of the different sonification types.

Background

Acquired brain lesions in adults, following stroke, head injury, or brain tumor, are major causes of acquired disability worldwide [11, 46]. These lesions induce multiple sensory, motor, and cognitive disorders. Among these disorders, motor impairments could affect 40% of patients after stroke [33]. In particular, we consider in this study the specific case of upper limb hemiparesis, characterized by impaired motor control and muscle weakness, greatly reduces autonomy in daily living activities, and thus, the long-term quality of life of patients [10].

The use of music is being studied in a wide range of rehabilitation settings [16, 39, 44, 47, 61], particularly in the case of acquired brain lesions [27, 68]. Several different methods have been investigated, such as audio-rhythmic stimulation (RAS) [65, 67], exercises with musical instruments (music-supported therapy) [1, 2, 13, 28, 52], and movement sonification devices [26, 36, 48, 58].

In this paper we focus on movement sonification, which concerns systems that enable translating in real-time motion parameters into sound or musical parameters [21, 30]. Movement sonification devices have many advantages [60]: access to a continuous 3D auditory information, fast adaptation of sound feedback to the movements performed, flexibility of use by participants with various profiles thanks to possible adaptation according to individual abilities [7, 23]. Thus, these devices present a potential added value in comparison with other sound/musical methods and tools, and offer perspectives in adequacy with the needs described in the rehabilitation framework [12, 66]. Moreover, compared to other feedback modalities such as visual feedback, the use of the auditory modality does not constrain the user's posture. In this case, the dependence to the external feedback, called the guidance effect, might be less important with auditory compared to visual feedback since sonification could encourage to focus attention on intrinsic proprioceptive information [19]. The potential interest of sonification devices as a rehabilitation support tool is therefore under investigation for different rehabilitation applications [25, 26, 37, 53, 63].

Concerning more specifically rehabilitation after acquired brain lesions, respectively in a pilot study [54] and a large-scale study [48], Schmitz et al. and Raglio et al. showed an encouraging evolution of the global dexterity scores (Box and Block Test) with sonification devices. In both situations, standard motor rehabilitation exercises were sonified.

In 2015, Scholz et al. proposed an innovative device where users learn to move in a virtual space associated with a musical scale, with the aim of playing melodies [57]. In this case, a decrease in pain scores was reported, as well as a trend towards improvement in the Stroke Impact Scale functional hand assessment scores. Nevertheless, this study did not show any improvement in scores on the other functional assessments performed (Action Research Arm Test, Box and Block Test, Nine Hole Peg Test). In a pilot study, Robertson et al. suggested that in the presence of audio feedback different results could be obtained depending on the hemispheric location of the brain lesion, and more precisely a deterioration in kinematic performances in the presence of audio feedback in the case of left hemispheric brain lesions [50].

Thus, although encouraging results have been obtained in different settings, limited functional benefits have also been reported [40]. One reason for contrasted effects could be related to the choice of sound and interaction design. In early works, the choice made was to sonify errors, by emitting “alarm” sounds when the participant does not follow the predicted trajectory model [36]. More recent approaches propose to favor the participant motivation and avoid negative reinforcement [7].

Moreover, the quality of the sound rendering has not always been a central concern, yet the choices of sound design and mapping could be fundamental to ensure the adequacy between the sound and the gesture to be performed, and thus, the effect of sonification on the movement control and learning [3, 15]. Questions about sound design and coupling modalities require further investigations [31]. In particular, these investigations must be considered with regard to the performed tasks, the user profiles they address, and individual singularities. Importantly, the need to consider multiple sonification modalities and to evaluate their effects was notably highlighted in two recent literature reviews [26, 41]. Moreover, the user perception and experience of sonification has been insufficiently studied.

In this perspective, the novelty of the present study was to evaluate, during a single session, different modalities of gesture-sound interactions, categories and types of sound feedback, with both adult patients with hemiparesis following an acquired brain lesion and healthy participants. An originality of our approach consists in using a mixed methodology, evaluating the effect of different sonification modalities both quantitatively and qualitatively. Our specific goal in this study was to measure the spontaneous effects of the sound feedback on the temporality of execution of the movement, and the subjective experience of each participant through semi-directed interviews. On the contrary to typical rehabilitation assessments where the motor task must be performed as quickly as possible (i.e. scores in assessments typically indexed on the number of objects moved [15, 17], or the number of repetitions of a movement or targets reached), we rather chose to give no instruction concerning the speed of execution of the task, and assess how the sonification could influence spontaneously the average movement speed.

Methods

Participants

All participants met the following inclusion criteria (Table 1): age between 18 and 80 years old, ability to understand the consent form and simple instructions, ability to answer questions during semi-structured interviews, and consent to participate.

Table 1 Description of the groups of participants

Participants were included in the patients group if they were hospitalized in rehabilitation department of Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital and had upper-limb hemiparesis after acquired brain lesion with sufficient recovery to initiate an elbow extension (without external help) and complete the motor task (stretch their elbow while sliding on a board). Exclusion criteria were any other neurological disease, severe neuropsychological impairments, musicogenic epilepsy, heart pacemaker, or hearing deficits requiring hearing aids.

Healthy volunteers were recruited and selected to be paired to the patient group considering sex and age range. Exclusion criteria were neurological pathology, upper-limb deficits of any origin, heart pacemaker, or hearing deficits requiring hearing aids.

Protocol

The experimental design consisted in three steps:

  1. 1.

    Interview and Amusia test

    An interview of each participant was carried out to evaluate their musical experience (vocal and/or instrumental education and practice), listening habits and possible hearing deficits. Three levels of musical expertise were distinguished: no musical practice, amateur experience or practice corresponding to a minimum of two years of regular vocal/instrumental training, and professional experience or practice. After the interview, their musical perception was assessed with the Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia (MBEA) [45]. The participants’ scores were compared to the norm established during the validation of the tool [45] in order to identify possible deviations from the norm in each group (Chi-square test). At last, their manual dominance was assessed with the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [42].

  2. 2.

    Sonification session

    During a single 1-hour sonification session the participants were instructed to extend the arm repetitively, sliding on the table with a fabric to minimize friction, following a straight trajectory. In order for the system to adapt to the motor skills of each participant, a personalized calibration was performed for each arm at the very beginning of the task to define both starting and end positions on the table that could be reached by the participant (see section Experimental setup). The instruction did not impose any particular timing to perform the movement: the participants were explicitly asked to perform the movement at the speed of their choice. This allowed us to compare average movement durations according to the participant profiles, while keeping the sound conditions order identical for all participants. During the session, participants used each arm alternatively, less-affected then affected for patients, and dominant then non-dominant for healthy subjects, with three different categories of sonification (direct sound modulation, musical interaction, and soundscape, described in detail in the section Experimental Setup) and no-sound condition. The order of presentation of the sound conditions starting on purpose from simple sound modulations, shown in Fig. 1, was identical for all participants, as we were aiming primarily to provide a comparable experience among participants. We included a no-sound condition at the beginning, the end, and between the three sonification categories, in order to assess the stability of the no sound-condition and any after-effect of each sound type on the no-sound conditions. This should allow us to ensure that the no-sound condition can be used as a participant-dependant control condition. Before each sound condition, a familiarization phase was performed to allow participants to understand each movement-sound coupling and stabilize their movement. After this phase, the participant performed ten repetitions. This number was advised by physiotherapists to avoid fatigue in patients.

  3. 3.

    After-session interview

    After the session of sonification a semi-structured interview of the subject experience was recorded with a dictaphone (Guide of semi-structured interview, on Additional file 1: S1). We also asked participants to sort by order of preference the sound conditions, and to choose in order 5 qualifying terms to describe their feeling in a 18 qualifier list, based on a balanced valence/arousal diagram.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Order of the sound conditions with the “no sound” condition intertwined No sound: N1, N2, N3, N4. Sound: Pi: Pitch, Dr: Drum, Md: Discrete Melody, Mc: Continuous Melody, Ss: Soundscape. These sonification types are described in detail in section Experimental Setup

Experimental setup

A specific system was built in order to provide sonification in response to the arm movement of the participant. Similarly to other recent proposed systems [4, 14, 59], we used wireless motion sensors, each containing an Inertial Measurement Units (IMU) 3D accelerometers, 3D gyroscope and 3D magnetometers, and transmits the data sampled at 200 Hz in real-time through WiFi. Precisely, three motion sensors were attached to both arms, as depicted in Fig. 2. The transmitted data include the orientation of the IMU units (i.e. the Euler angles), which are used to compute a normalized displacement parameter after performing a calibration consisting in recording the IMUs data at the start and stop positions (indicated in Fig. 2A). This displacement parameter is used as the input parameter in the sonification system (described in Section Sonification strategies). As mentioned in the protocol, the calibration procedure was performed at the beginning of each participant's session, which allows for adapting the sonification to each participant's motor capabilities, since the actual arm displacement can be different for each one.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Experimental Setup. A placement of the motion sensors (IMU). B Schematic of the dataflow process and auditory output. C Basic description of the computation of the displacement using the two calibration points indicated as start and stop (median plane)

The laptop computer, connected to a soundcard and speakers, operates the sonification using a program written with Max7 (Cycling’74) and the extension MuBu for Max [56]. This library allows for performing data signal processing, and controlled sound synthesis. The software is also used to record all the IMUs data to the computer, along with the sounds. The audio rendering system was composed of one stereo speaker in front and two additional mono speakers in the back of the participant in order to create an immersive sound environment.

The displacement data, along with the raw IMUs data and audio output were recorded during all movement cycles, and saved in the computer. Video recordings were also performed during all the experiments, allowing the verification of the data collected with the IMUs.

A data analysis script (Matlab, R2018a, Mathworks USA) was developed allowing for data visualization, and semi-automatic segmentation of the displacement data. From this, we computed the total duration time for each movement cycle (see Fig. 3). This led us to compute 5 different time duration: extension duration, plateau-1 duration, return duration, plateau-2 duration, and total duration time (sum of the 4 previous ones).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Normalized displacement over time with the different computed phases. 1 The “elongation phase” of the upper-limb (extension of the elbow), 2 The “plateau-1”: phase of maintenance in a position of maximum upper-limb elongation, 3 the “retraction phase”, return to the initial position, 4 the “plateau-2” phase in the initial retracted position, elbow bent, before initiating a new extension-flexion cycle

Sonification design

We decided to implement 5 different types of sound, classified in 3 different sonification categories, in order to evaluate how different sounds and musical interactions could influence the movement timing and how they were perceived by the users in this context. Audios and sound spectrograms are presented in Additional file 1: S2.

  1. 1.

    Direct sound modulation

    This category of sonification has been largely implemented in sonification systems and reported in the literature [18, 41].

    Pitch (Pi): direct relationship between the reaching distance and the pitch. In order to avoid the annoyance of a pure tone, we use granular synthesis in order to vary the pitch of a sample sound that contains a rich spectrum (from 92 to 500 Hz). The farther the reaching point, the higher the pitch. The range of variation of the fundamental frequency is from 92 to 122 Hz, with a strong harmonic varying from 279 to 376 Hz.

    Drum (Dr): direct relationship between the reaching distance and the tempo of a regular beat pulsation. We used a drum sound, with a regular rhythmic pattern (such as 4 eighth notes). The farther the reaching point, the faster the tempo. The range of variation is from 3.2 to 16 Hz beats. At the fast tempo, the drum sounds like a drum roll.

  2. 2.

    Melody playing

    Sonification implying music can potentially be motivating for the participant as shown previously [26]. In this paradigm, the user can play a melody by moving the arm. All the notes are programmed, so the task consists in activating the progression of the melody. Two distinct cases were implemented:

    Music/Discrete (Md): a full forward arm movement triggers a “discrete melody”, following a tonal harmonic progression (based on Concerto No. 5 in F Minor, BWV 1056). The movement triggers a different part of the melody at each outward and backward movements (four notes per outward or backward). This sonification was previously used in a music education scenario [29].

    Music/continuous (Mc): a full arm movement enables one to continuously “play” a complete musical phrase, using the so-called gesture follower technique, which has been used in music pedagogy [5, 6]. In this system, a time progression index of the gesture is estimated by comparing the performed gesture with a displacement profile recorded previously. Then, this estimated time progression index is used to trigger notes of the melody. The piece was a record of the Prelude in C Major by J.S. Bach interpreted by Glenn Gould.

  3. 3.

    Environmental sounds triggering

    This sonification category is based on everyday listening, invoking recognizable sound environments [34]. In this paradigm, the reaching movement is divided in three different zones, each one being associated with a specific environmental sound, called ‘soundscapes’.

    Soundscape (Ss): The reaching movement is divided in three equal parts. Each one triggers, respectively, sounds of wind, river and birds.

Data analysis

Movement analysis

The statistical analyses were performed with JMP software® (SAS Inc., Cary, NC, USA). All tests were 2-sided. A p-value ≤ 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

A first step consisted in testing the homogeneity of the “no sound” conditions using analysis of variance (ANOVA) on repeated measures (or Friedman non parametric test on ranks when underlying assumptions were not verified), to evaluate any order effect in the no-sound sequences. Differences between these sequences being non significant, data were normalized by dividing duration values by the average of the no sound values in order to take into account inter-individual variability. Importantly, the normalization is performed for each arm, considering always identical distances between sound and no sound durations. The normalized time is thus equivalent to a ratio between sound and no sound durations (which could be alternatively expressed in %). Then, for each participant (patient or healthy participants) and arm considered, Student paired t test (or Wilcoxon signed-rank test), Anova on repeated measures (or Friedman test) and mixed linear models were performed on normalized data taking into account sound context. This was followed, when needed, by post-hoc Tukey HSD analysis (or Durbin-Conover test). When parametric tests were applied we made sure that the underlying assumptions (normality, homoscedasticity or sphericity for repeated measures) were valid.

Participant’s experience analysis

In order to obtain the average preference of sound and qualifying terms used by the participants, we associated 1 to 5 points (5 being the preferred) to each sound or qualifying term for each individual ranking order. We then calculated the average points for each sound or terms.

The audio recording of each semi-structured interview has been transcribed verbatim. Three experimenters (IP, BC, FB) carried out the thematic analysis of the transcribed interviews [8]. Each experimenter read the transcription and generated individual codings from the participant’s interviews. The experimenters then gathered the codes and kept the common codes or the ones that may not be common to the three experimenters, but that reached a consensus after discussions. From the selected codes, we defined thematic axes, and we kept a list of illustrative quotations for each axis. For each result we distinguish particularities of each group of participants (designed as “P” concerning patients, and “H” concerning healthy participants).

Results

Participants description

Two groups of subjects participated and followed the protocol entirely: 15 patients with motor deficit (hemiparesia) resulting from acquired brain lesions, and 15 healthy participants were included.

Descriptive information about gender, age, musical background, MBEA scores, and side of hemiparesia are reported in Table 1. More details about each participant are reported on Additional file 1: S3 (Descriptive data of participant’s profiles).

All participants were right-handed, and gender repartition, age and musical background are similar between groups. Motor impairments were evaluated when patients were admitted to the hospital, and were not reliable at the time of the inclusion in the experiment. For time constraints, it was not possible to perform other motor assessments before the experiment. The design of the normalization procedure was aimed at reducing the influence of the different levels of motor impairments. This point will be again specified in the results, discussion and conclusion.

Comparative analysis of MBEA scores revealed lower scores in patient groups than in healthy ones (Additional file 1: S4). This difference is at the limit of significance (test Chi2 p = 0.05).

Movement data results

Individual data

Data without normalization of the averages of the total durations performed by each individual with no sound (N1, N2, N3 and N4) and with sonification (Pi, Dr, Md, Mc, Ss), are presented in Fig. 4, considering the subject group (patient and healthy participants) and the arm (paretic side vs. less affected, and dominant vs. non-dominant).

Fig. 4
figure 4

No sound vs sound condition, for patients and healthy participants. The error bars correspond to the 95% confidence intervals

In Fig. 4, it appears that the average duration of a complete cycle varies across participants. This result must be considered in regards to the fact that we did not give any timing constraint on the movement performance. This is also probably linked to different motor ability, especially in the patients with different levels of motor impairment.

We also observe that there are more variations in the sound conditions compared to the no sound conditions. More precisely, as shown in Additional file 1: S5, the four “no sound” conditions were compared for all participants, and no significant difference was found. Therefore, this stability confirmed that the “no sound” condition can be used for normalizing each participant's sound conditions measurement.

Comparison between the sound and no sound conditions

The comparative analysis of the average of total cycle durations with no sound compared to sonification shows a significant difference for the normalized total time, for each situation considered (p < 0.001 for both arms in patients and healthy participants) (Fig. 5). Specifically, the total average duration increased with sonification compared to cases without sound feedback.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Mean for the patients (left) and healthy participants (right), for all the sound and no sound conditions, considering the different arms (affected / less affected for the patients, and non-dominant/dominant for the healthy participants). The error bars correspond to the 95% confidence intervals

If we consider in more details the duration of the different phases constituting the extension-flexion cycles of the elbow, we observe significant differences in the average duration of plateau-1 (phase of maximum elongation), retraction phase, and plateau-2 (phase of minimum elongation between two extension phases), when comparing sonification to no sound conditions for both groups. Plateau-1 and the return phase were longer whatever the arm considered both in patients and healthy participants (p < 0.001). Thus, participants remained in maximal extension for a longer time with sonification compared to no sound condition and returned to the starting point slower in the presence of sonification compared to the no sound condition. The average duration of plateau-2 were also longer both in patients for paretic and less-affected arm (p < 0.05 and p < 0.001, respectively) and in healthy participants for non-dominant and dominant arm (p < 0.005 and p < 0.05, respectively).

Concerning the extension phase, significant differences between the sonification and no sound conditions were observed in the healthy participants for both arms and in patients for the less affected arm (p < 0.001).

Comparison between each sound condition

The comparative analysis of the average of total cycle durations according to the sound feedback categories (Anova on repeated measures) shows significant differences between sonifications categories, in the case of the paretic arms of patients and the dominant arms of healthy participants (p < 0.05) (Additional file 1: S6). These significant differences are not found in the other situations (less affected arms of patients and non-dominant arms of controls).

Results concerning the participant’s experience

Sound preferences and Experience qualifiers

We report the individual rankings, rated from 1 to 5 points, according to the hierarchical preference of the sound feedback (Table 2) and the qualifying terms respectively (Table 3). The most appreciated sound feedback by both groups of participants is the soundscape, followed by the continuous music. Among the 18 qualifiers list, playful is the first shared term for both groups of participants to qualify their experience.

Table 2 Sound feedback rankings
Table 3 Experience’s qualifiers rankings

Thematic analysis based on semi-directive interviews

The thematic analysis revealed six common themes across subject groups.

Reported feelings using the system:

First, the majority of participants reported feeling a difference in the sound context when performing the task (24/30; 14 Patients—10 Healthy), and they preferred performing the gestures in the presence of sonification (29/30; 14P-15H).

Second, the task performance was not considered to be more difficult with sonification than without in the majority of cases (22/30; 11P-11H). Thirteen participants (7P-6H) even reported that it was easier to perform the forearm extension task with sonification. Nevertheless, six participants (2P-4H) reported the experience being occasionally more difficult with some specific couplings, which they justify with two different reasons: a mismatch between the sound type and the gesture to be performed (Md), and when a specific movement quality was required for the sound production (Mc). Concerning the first aspect, several participants underlined that the jerky sound of the discontinuous melody was not matching with the representation of a regular gesture to be carried out. That induced a desire to adapt the gesture in relation to the produced sound, which implied then to perform it in a jerky way. Concerning the second aspect, the participants specified that Mc generates an expectation for the quality of the music produced. In turn, this would require a finer motor control (H01): "Since it’s music, I want it to sound like something fluid that one could listen to".

Finally, one participant mentioned a notable distinction between perception and volition (P04): "The impressions were not different but the intentions could be". This participant reported a deeper involvement in the task performance in the presence of the sonification: "[…] The movement is more voluntary when it produces a sound".

Sound as cues:

Looking more specifically at the interaction between movement parameters and sound coupling, the participants spontaneously mentioned a notion of “cues”, as guides for the movement. This appeared recurrently while mentioning various movement characteristics: amplitude, fluidity, regularity and reproducibility.

Among the characteristics mentioned, temporal aspects were very predominant. Several types of the sonifications effects were described with respect to the temporal characteristics of movement and sound, such as a modulation of the feeling of time (P07): "I had the impression that when I perform a movement with the sound I took more time, I went less quickly to do it", the temporal reference mark (P09): "With music we have a reference point, we keep the same cruising pace", or the more conscious search for an adaptation to the representation conveyed by the sound, in order to obtain a certain sound quality (H08): "When there was no sound I always performed at the same speed, when there was sound I varied the speeds a little because I wanted it to fit with the sound".

Perceived interaction modalities:

For a minority of participants, the interaction modality was unidirectional: four of them felt that the sounds led the movement (4 participants, 3P-1H), and four other participants felt that the gesture controlled the sounds (or vice versa that the sounds followed the gestures, 3P-1H). Other participants (4 patients) expressed having experienced a feedback loop. According to them, the gestures triggered the sounds which in turn provided them with feedback on the gestures, allowing them to adapt to the perceived sound/music.

Finally, for the majority of the participants, the experience of the interaction varied and evolved during the experiment (18 participants; 5P-13H) according to 3 main parameters: the type of sound feedback (1P-6H), the arm performing the task (3P), and the evolution of their understanding of the functioning of the system during the experiment (3H). Regarding the categories, types of sound feedback, and the proposed couplings, participants unanimously expressed that the gestures controlled the sounds for the simple couplings (Pitch and Drum) while the gestures adapted to the sounds for the musical couplings, especially the continuous melody (Mc). Regarding the way the arm performing the task affects the experience of the interaction, patients specified that, for the paretic arm, the gesture controlled the sound, whereas, with the less affected arm, the sound controlled the gesture, or that the gesture adapted to the sounds. Finally, regarding the evolution during the experiment of the interaction understanding, participants expressed that they followed the sound at first, and that later they voluntarily controlled their gesture in order to modulate the sound. H13: "At the beginning I had the impression that I was trying to follow the sound…well to make a gesture following the rhythm, and then I understood that I could control the sound myself with the gesture".

All of these findings suggest that the nature of the sound feedback and the coupling modalities had an influence on the perception of the interaction and on the participants’ experience. H06: “The coupling between the sound and the movement changes the experience of the movement, and so even if you’re trying to do the same movement, even if it’s exactly the same movement, the way you experience it is different, the involvement of the person in the task is really changed”.

Reported emotions

Some participants spontaneously stated that the task was more enjoyable, funnier, more engaging, and more interesting with any type of sonification. Four main affective states were expressed by the participants: playfulness, curiosity, frustration and relaxation. The notion of playfulness is predominant in the spontaneous comments of the participants (8P-9H). Many participants also mentioned their curiosity and surprise at discovering the device. This surprise was often at the origin of the playfulness mentioned above. P07: "I was surprised by the sounds I was making when I was doing the acceleration and deceleration movements. It surprised me, and I liked it, I found it very playful". In other cases, the curiosity was formalized by expecting something from the device. Frustration could also emerge in reaction to the restrictive framework of the instruction: H08: “The fact that I could only do one movement of extension of the arm is a little frustrating because I would have done other movements […] me in any case I wanted to adapt my movements to the sounds”. Finally, the notion of relaxation was expressed many times by the participants, more particularly regarding two couplings: the 'continuous melody' and the ‘soundscape’, implying in some cases body feeling and the task performance. P09: “With the music it softens, it soothes, it’s like we were being massaged, as if we were being put in a second state to be willing. At one point there was music with the sea, the wind, it relaxes you, when you are obliged to make a movement and you can't do it, it relaxes you”.

Mental imagery

Many participants associated the gesture-sound couplings with different mental imagery. The ‘pitch’ was associated with images of a vinyl record, a soft car engine, an ocean or even described as celestial. The 'drum' has been associated with muffled hammering or African drums. The 'discontinuous melody' has been the object of less and contrasted associations (mandolin, stalactic in a cave), although images of bouncing movements have been widely mentioned. The 'continuous melody', which original musical piece was sometimes recognized and named, was associated with the idea of spring, and 'dream-space'. This sonic coupling, in some cases, created the illusion of being a musician (H13): 'I caught myself for thirty seconds as if I were Mozart, so I was very pleased with myself'. Finally, the 'soundscape', a metaphorical space by design, was the most prolific in terms of images, very often associated with the idea of escaping. H09: "There were images that appeared, […] I imagined a kind of walk in a forest, we walk next to the river, then we arrive in a meadow, where there are birds… we imagine the scenery that goes with it".

The stimulation of mental imagery is linked to the participants' preferences: the more the person appreciates the coupling, the more his or her mental imagery is triggered and stimulated. P08: "Every time there was music, I imagined a scene or a moment that I experienced. Especially on the music that I liked in fact ".

Attentional modulation

Evocating the feeling of escape, as well as various emotions, led several participants to report having felt a modulation of their attention during the task, and this depending on the sound context. However, differences across groups should be highlighted.

In the control group, the majority of participants mentioned that their attention was mainly focused on the sounds (10H) H01: "When there was a sound I was thinking less about the movement, I was thinking less about reaching out, I was focusing on the sound". For the other five participants in this group, they could either focus their attention simultaneously or alternately on the sound and the gestures. One participant specified the effects of the feedback loop on their attention and evoked the notion of embodiment: "The attention is not on the movement itself, but on the movement in the context of the effects it has on the music, so I think it changes a lot our way of thinking about the body during the movement".

Within the group of patients, the comments were more contrasted: 4 expressed that their attention was rather focused on the sounds, 4 rather on the gestures, while the others mentioned that the focus of the attention varied, either according to their appreciation of the coupling, or according to the arm performing the task. Indeed, as the gesture could be difficult to perform with their paretic arm, the attention could then shift to the gesture, while being supported by the sound: P06: "[affected side] we are very preoccupied by the very basic movement we have to do. The extension is difficult so we focus on the movement. When I liked the sounds, the attention was directed to the sounds".

Discussion

Temporality: with sound versus without sound feedback

Our goal was to evaluate the spontaneous effect of sonification on temporal parameters and user experience, without giving any temporal constraints to the participants, as stated in the instruction that we provided where the gesture timing was left free.

The results we obtained suggest that, whatever the status of the participants (patients with sequelae of an acquired brain injury, or healthy participants) and the arm considered (paretic vs. less affected or dominant vs. non-dominant), the presence of a sound feedback has an effect on the participant’s feeling during the experience and on the gesture performance timing of the extension-retraction of the elbow on a table.

While we observed large inter-individual differences, partially due to heterogeneous motor ability and/or impairments, the normalization procedure enabled us to highlight a significant global slowing down of the movement in the presence of sound feedback, with in particular a longer duration of maintenance in maximum extension and minimal extension, and a slowing down of the return phase in all the situations considered. This observation of a longer duration of maximum extension in the presence of sound feedback is encouraging as to the possibility of using it in a rehabilitative context, in order to prolong the duration of a posture maintenance during stretching exercises, with the aim of promoting a progressive gain in the amplitude of movement.

Different mechanisms that could shed light on the reasons for the differences in temporality of movement in the presence of sound feedback can be considered, regarding the participants' comments and the literature.

A first hypothesis would be that the induced attentional load may have contributed to the slowing down of the movement in the presence of sound feedback. However, this hypothesis does not seem to be in agreement with the analysis of the qualitative results and the literature. Indeed, the participants indicated that the sound feedback worked as cues, allowing them to pace the movement, and this even when no tempo or intrinsic rhythmic element was present in the sound feedback (cases of the pitch and the soundscape). The presence of sound feedback can suggest an implicit timing. For example, in the case of the continuous musical sound feedback (Mc), although the speed remained free, the implicit tempo of the piece could suggest a movement speed. In this regard, Sihvonen et al. [61] suggests that in the presence of sound feedback participants make inferences about the timing of sound events, consequently influencing the temporality of movement completion. The repetition of a movement at a regular and constant tempo with audio feedback would thus be likely to induce its automation, and the attentional system could be less solicited thanks to this temporal cueing function. Further, research on attentional processes mobilized during motor learning has shown that external focus induces a more automatic control, less costly, and therefore beneficial for the realization of the movement [24]. From this perspective, assuming that sound feedback are sources of external focus and implicit learning, their use should therefore allow for limiting attentional load, provided that the design is adapted [19]. Also, the observed slowing of movement would therefore not be attributable to an attentional overhead. Nevertheless, in the case of our experiment, as this was the first use of a motion sonification device for all participants (both healthy and patients), it cannot be totally excluded that other processes were involved. In particular, it is more usual for novices to adopt a strategy of attentional focus on internal parameters and explicit learning [24]. Although the intrinsic principle of sonification devices is conducive to external focus and implicit learning [19], the attentional processes mobilized during the use of sonification devices remain insufficiently known to date. In particular, it would be necessary to study the strategies used according to the users' experience in order to determine in which cases sound feedback can be considered as distractors [35, 43], sources of external focus, or even sources of internal focus if we consider that an optimized mapping could be likely to favor attention to proprioception. In the case of rehabilitation, it is commonly accepted that it is important to limit attentional distractors and that dual-task situations can be too costly and diminish motor performance in the case of gait [38]. However, decentering participants' attention during the execution of a motor task can, under certain conditions, also improve its completion [32]. In this perspective, investigations centered on the mobilization of the attention aroused by sonification in the case of rehabilitation should be carried out. On this topic, the comments of the participants in our study suggest differences in strategies between individuals. Some participants mentioned focusing on the sound source, others focusing on the movement, or an oscillation in the source of attentional focus, navigating between internal (movement) and external focus (sound), depending in particular on the sound feedback used, or even in some cases a joint attention to the different sources.

A complementary approach corresponds to considering sound feedback as information contributing to internal models of movement control. Based on work on motor control and learning [21, 22], proposed to consider motion sonification from the perspective of multisensory integration theory. Under a reserve of few conditions (design adequacy and sound mapping) the effects of motion sonification would not be solely related to rhythmic adaptation. Building on the work of Rauschecker [49] Effenberg et al. [23] and Schmitz et al. [55] clarified that, in a manner comparable to the processing of visual information, two dissociated pathways for the processing of auditory perceptual information should be considered: the conscious ventral pathway (“what”) and the unconscious dorsal pathway (“where”). According to these authors, the dorsal pathway, which is unconscious and particularly important for motor control, could be brought into play during the sonification of the movement according to the design and the sound mapping. Thus, the auditory information related to the movement transmitted during the sonification would contribute to the improvement of the sensorimotor representations, and to the internal models, by being processed at a non-conscious level.

In the case of our study, the participants reported that, beyond an impression of modulation of the movement speed, the sound feedback exerted a more global influence on their volition, their intention and their implication in the movement. We could therefore suppose that a conscious processing also took place during the task performance, along with a modulation of the sense of agency. Beyond an effect on the physiological parameters of the movement, the movement sonification was shown to possibly modify the participants’ body perception and representation [62]. By extension, movement sonification could therefore modify their relationship to their movement by diverting them from a functional goal to an aesthetic one [64]. In our study, the considered task does not involve a functional goal as in the case of pointing or grasping an object. The presence of a sound feedback thus provides the participant with a goal for the task, allowing the transition from a simple repetition task to a goal-oriented task, we can refer to a “sound-oriented” goal [7]. The presence of a sound feedback thus modifies the intentionality of the gesture. Moreover, the interactive process influences the participants’ perception of their movement control, and allows them to playfully experiment situations, alternating between sensations of producing or following sounds. In this perspective, this modulation of the sense of agency in the presence of sound feedback, especially reinforced during the first use of a sonification device, could also explain the global slowing down of the movement.

In order to shed light on the processes (neurophysiological, perceptual, attentional and cognitive) involved in the execution of simple gestures with a movement sonification device, further studies are necessary.

Specificity of the sound feedback

The comparative analysis of the temporal movement data reveals significant differences according to the category of sound considered in the cases of paretic arms of patients and dominant arms of healthy participants. No significant differences were found in the other cases, and concerning comparative analysis of each type of sound condition. However, similarities of distribution profiles encourage deepening investigations, considering our small sample size (15 participants in each group) induced a low statistical power. Also, it would be necessary to randomize the order of presentation of sound feedback to further describe a possible differentiating effect of each sound. In addition, very contrasting feelings according to the types of sound feedback were expressed in the participants' interviews and also support the interest to investigate further the effect of each sound feedback on movement timing.

The temporal dimension of a sound feedback could indeed influence the movement performance differently. In this study, Drum displays explicit timing information (direct variation of a pulse according to the extension of the elbow) while discrete and continuous music (Md, Mc) displays implicit timing information. By implicit timing information, we refer to cases where the participants try to adapt their movement to render the musical extract as they anticipate, using prior knowledge. The intrinsic temporal and aesthetic sonification characteristics seems to influence the movement performance timing and feeling, which was previously reported in the literature [19]. In particular it seems that a higher musical quality, as in the case of the continuous melody, was perceived as a motivational added value, even if it imposes larger constraints on the movement performance.

The participants' interviews also pointed towards the notion of affordance. In the case of sounds, affordance can be defined as the opportunities for actions elicited by a sound [20], in other words, the sound characteristics eliciting a representation of an associated movement [9] . In our case, the discontinuous sound feedback (Md) was indeed associated with the desire to perform bouncing motions, rather than a continuous sliding motion. These remarks underline the intrinsic link between representations of movements associated with sound feedback and the necessity to take them into consideration during the sound design for a correct adequacy between the characteristics of the proposed sound and the motor task to be performed. It is very interesting to note that, beyond the category of sound feedback, the specific characteristics of each sound feedback are likely to influence the participants’ feelings and emotional states. Precisely, the specificities of each “musical” condition induce different feelings and a notable preference. These observations are in line with the literature [19, 51] and therefore support the hypothesis that the nature of the sound feedback used, its characteristics, the sound design, and the coupling modalities, influence the movement timing and participant’s experience.

Conclusion, limitations and perspectives

In conclusion, the sonification has a significant effect on the temporal execution of the movement during a single-session. This effect was established for both healthy participants and patients with upper-limb hemiparesia after acquired brain lesion. Moreover, qualitative analysis pointed out that performing the task with sonification changes participants' feelings, notably concerning intentionality, volition and motivation during movement.

Specificities and intrinsic characteristics of each type of sound feedback and gesture-sound coupling could be likely to influence the effect of sonification on the temporality of the gesture and its experience. The majority of participants have a preference for the soundscape and musical feedback, under reserve of congruence with the gesture to perform. Special attention must be paid to the potential difficulty induced and the emotions likely to be felt. The qualitative results suggest that it would be interesting to investigate attentional processes mobilized by the sonification modalities with multiple motor tasks and various participants profiles, and the possible evolution of the attentional cost according to the training.

Beyond the limitations present in this study (limited sample, motor impairments heterogeneity, single session without follow-up over several sessions, no randomization of the order of presentation of sound feedback, focus on the temporal criterion in the analysis of movement data), it contributes to current open methodological questions concerning the evaluation of movement sonification, specifically in the context of rehabilitation.

This study calls for further investigations. A first question concerns the most efficient use of sonification, whether an immediate and spontaneous effect on movement performance with sound is finally preferable to the results of progressive learning with long-term training. Second, it remains to better establish relevant parameters or criteria (physiological, functional, attentional, motivational) for the evaluation of the effectiveness of a movement sonification system. Third, it seems important to evaluate any potential unconscious effect of the sonification of movement on voluntary motor skills.

Availability of data and materials

The data sets and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

MBEA:

Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia

IMU:

Inertial Measurement Unit

N:

No Sound

Pi:

Pitch

Dr:

Drum

Md:

Music discrete

Mc:

Music continuous

Ss:

Soundscape

P:

Patients

H:

Healthy participants

ANOVA:

Analysis of Variance

References

  1. Altenmüller E, Marco-Pallares J, Münte TF, Schneider S. Neural reorganization underlies improvement in stroke-induced motor dysfunction by music-supported therapy. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2009;1169(1):395–405.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Amengual JL, Rojo N, Veciana de Las Heras M, Marco-Pallarés J, Grau-Sánchez J, Schneider S, et al. Sensorimotor plasticity after music-supported therapy in chronic stroke patients revealed by transcranial magnetic stimulation. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(4): e61883.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Avanzini F, Spagnol S, Rodà A, De Götzen A. Designing interactive sound for motor rehabilitation tasks. Sonic Interact Des. 2013. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.7551/mitpress/8555.003.0017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Berner K, Cockcroft J, Morris LD, Louw Q. Concurrent validity and within-session reliability of gait kinematics measured using an inertial motion capture system with repeated calibration. J Bodyw Mov Ther. 2020;24(4):251–60.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Bevilacqua F, Guédy F, Schnell N, Fléty E, Leroy N. Wireless sensor interface and gesture-follower for music pedagogy. In: Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on New Interfaces for Musical Expression-NIME ’07, 124. 2007.

  6. Bevilacqua F, Zamborlin B, Sypniewski A, Schnell N, Guédy F, Rasamimanana N. Continuous realtime gesture following and recognition. In: Kopp S, Wachsmuth I, editors. Gesture in embodied communication and human-computer interaction. Berlin: Springer; 2010. p. 73–84.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Bevilacqua F, Boyer EO, Françoise J, Houix O, Susini P, Roby-Brami A, et al. Sensori-motor learning with movement sonification: perspectives from recent interdisciplinary studies. Front Neurosci. 2016. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3389/fnins.2016.00385.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Braun V, Clarke V. Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual Res Psychol. 2006;3(2):77–101.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Caramiaux B, Françoise J, Schnell N, Bevilacqua F. Mapping through listening. Comput Music J. 2014;38:34–48.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Cerniauskaite M, Quintas R, Koutsogeorgou E, Meucci P, Sattin D, Leonardi M, et al. Quality-of-life and disability in patients with stroke. Am J Phys Med Rehabil. 2012;91(13):39–47.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Chan V, Zagorski B, Parsons D, Colantonio A. Older adults with acquired brain injury: a population based study. BMC Geriatr. 2013;13(1):97.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Chang WH. Personalized approaches to stroke: one step forward for functional recovery of stroke patients. J Pers Med. 2022;12(5):822.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Chen JL. Music-supported therapy for stroke motor recovery: theoretical and practical considerations. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2018. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/nyas.13726.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Cho YS, Jang SH, Cho JS, Kim MJ, Lee HD, Lee SY, et al. Evaluation of validity and reliability of inertial measurement unit-based gait analysis systems. Ann Rehabil Med. 2018;42(6):872–83.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Croarkin E, Danoff J, Barnes C. Evidence-based rating of upper-extremity motor function tests used for people following a stroke. Phys Ther. 2004;84(1):62–74.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. de Dreu MJ, van der Wilk ASD, Poppe E, Kwakkel G, van Wegen EEH. Rehabilitation, exercise therapy and music in patients with Parkinson’s disease: a meta-analysis of the effects of music-based movement therapy on walking ability, balance and quality of life. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2012;18:114–9.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Desrosiers J, Bravo G, Hébert R, Dutil É, Mercier L. Validation of the box and block test as a measure of dexterity of elderly people: reliability, validity, and norms studies. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1994;75(7):751–5.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Dubus G, Bresin R. A systematic review of mapping strategies for the sonification of physical quantities. PLOS ONE. 2013. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1371/journal.pone.0082491.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Dyer JF, Stapleton P, Rodger MWM. Sonification as concurrent augmented feedback for motor skill learning and the importance of mapping design. Open Psychol J. 2015;8(1):192–202.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Dyer JF. Human movement sonification for motor skill learning [PhD Thesis]. Queen’s University Belfast. Faculty of Engineering and Physical Sciences. 2017.

  21. Effenberg AO. Movement sonification: effects on perception and action. IEEE Multimedia. 2005;12(2):53–9.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Effenberg AO, Fehse U, Weber A. Movement sonification: audiovisual benefits on motor learning. BIO Web Conf. 2011;1:00022.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Effenberg AO, Fehse U, Schmitz G, Krueger B, Mechling H. Movement sonification: effects on motor learning beyond rhythmic adjustments. Front Neurosci. 2016;10:219.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Ferrel-Chapus C, Tahej PK. Processus attentionnels et apprentissage moteur. Sci Mot. 2010;71:71–83.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Friedman N, Chan V, Reinkensmeyer AN, Beroukhim A, Zambrano GJ, Bachman M, et al. Retraining and assessing hand movement after stroke using the MusicGlove: comparison with conventional hand therapy and isometric grip training. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2014;11(1):76.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Ghai S. Effects of real-time (sonification) and rhythmic auditory stimuli on recovering arm function post stroke: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Neurol. 2018;9:488.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Ghai S, Ghai I. Effects of (music-based) rhythmic auditory cueing training on gait and posture post-stroke: a systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis. Sci Rep. 2019;9(1):2183.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Grau-Sánchez J, Segura E, Sanchez-Pinsach D, Raghavan P, Münte TF, Palumbo AM, Turry A, Duarte E, Särkämö T, Cerquides J, Arcos JL, Rodríguez-Fornells A. Enriched music-supported therapy for chronic stroke patients: a study protocol of a randomised controlled trial. BMC Neurol. 2021;21(1):19.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Guédy F, Bevilacqua F, Fober D, Robert L. Etudier et jouer une œuvre musicale en manipulant des objets. Journées d’Informatique Musicale. 2013.

  30. Hermann T, Hunt A, Neuhoff JG. The sonification handbook. Berlin: Logos Verlag; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Kantan PR, Spaich EG, Dahl S. A metaphor-based technical framework for musical sonification in movement rehabilitation. In: Proceedings of the 26th International Conference on Auditory Display (ICAD 2021). Virtual Conference: International Community for Auditory Display. 2021; 176‑83.

  32. Kim SA, Ryu YU, Shin HK. The effects of different attentional focus on poststroke gait. J Exerc Rehabil. 2019;15(4):592–6.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Lecoffre C, de Peretti C, Gabet A, Grimaud O, Woimant F, Giroud M, et al. National trends in patients hospitalized for stroke and stroke mortality in France, 2008 to 2014. Stroke. 2017;48(11):2939–45.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Lemaitre G, Grimault N, Suied C. Acoustics and psychoacoustics of sound scenes and events. In: Virtanen T, Plumbley M, Ellis D, editors. Computational analysis of sound scenes and events. Cham: Springer; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Liu W, Magalhaes MA, Mackay WE, Beaudouin-Lafon M, Bevilacqua F. Motor variability in complex gesture learning: effects of movement sonification and musical background. ACM Trans Appl Percept. 2022;19(1):1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Maulucci RA, Eckhouse RH. Retraining reaching in chronic stroke with real-time auditory feedback. NRE. 2001;16(3):171–82.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Mezzarobba S, Grassi M, Pellegrini L, Catalan M, Krüger B, Stragapede L, et al. Action observation improves sit-to-walk in patients with Parkinson’s disease and freezing of gait. Biomechanical analysis of performance. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2020;80:133–7.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Montero-Odasso M, Verghese J, Beauchet O, Hausdorff JM. Gait and cognition: a complementary approach to understanding brain function and the risk of falling. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60(11):2127–36.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Moumdjian L, Sarkamo T, Leone C, Leman M, Feys P. Effectiveness of music-based interventions on motricity or cognitive functioning in neurological populations: a systematic review. Eur J Phys Rehabil Med. 2016. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.23736/S1973-9087.16.04429-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Nikmaram N, Scholz DS, Großbach M, Schmidt SB, Spogis J, Belardinelli P, Müller-Dahlhaus F, Remy J, Ziemann U, Rollnik JD, Altenmüller E. Musical sonification of arm movements in stroke rehabilitation yields limited benefits. Front Neurosci. 2019;13:1378.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Nown TH, Upadhyay P, Kerr A, Andonovic I, Tachtatzis C, Grealy MA. A mapping review of real-time movement sonification systems for movement rehabilitation. IEEE Rev Biomed Eng. 2023. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1109/RBME.2022.3187840.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Parmentier FBR. The cognitive determinants of behavioral distraction by deviant auditory stimuli: a review. Psychol Res. 2014;78(3):321–38.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Pereira APS, Marinho V, Gupta D, Magalhães F, Ayres C, Teixeira S. Music therapy and dance as gait rehabilitation in patients with Parkinson disease: a review of evidence. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2019;32(1):49–56.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Peretz I, Champod AS, Hyde K. Varieties of musical disorders the montreal battery of evaluation of Amusia. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2003;999:58–75.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Pollock A, Farmer SE, Brady MC, Langhorne P, Mead GE, Mehrholz J, et al. Interventions for improving upper limb function after stroke (Review). Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2014. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/14651858.CD010820.pub2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Raglio A, Oasi O, Gianotti M, Rossi A, Goulene K, Stramba-Badiale M. Improvement of spontaneous language in stroke patients with chronic aphasia treated with music therapy: a randomized controlled trial. Int J Neurosci. 2016;126(3):235–42.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Raglio A, Panigazzi M, Colombo R, Tramontano M, Iosa M, Mastrogiacomo S, et al. Hand rehabilitation with sonification techniques in the subacute stage of stroke. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):7237.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Rauschecker JP. An expanded role for the dorsal auditory pathway in sensorimotor control and integration. Hear Res. 2011;271(1–2):16–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Robertson JV, Hoellinger T, Lindberg P, Bensmail D, Hanneton S, Roby-Brami A. Effect of auditory feedback differs according to side of hemiparesis: a comparative pilot study. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2009;6(1):45.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Roddy S. Mapping for meaning: the embodied sonification listening model and its implications for the mapping problem in sonic information design. J Multimodal User Interfaces. 2020. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s12193-020-00318-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Rojo N, Amengual J, Juncadella M, Rubio F, Camara E, Marco-Pallares J, et al. Music-supported therapy induces plasticity in the sensorimotor cortex in chronic stroke: a single-case study using multimodal imaging (fMRI-TMS). Brain Inj. 2011;25(7–8):787–93.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Schaffert N, Janzen TB, Mattes K, Thaut MH. A review on the relationship between sound and movement in sports and rehabilitation. Front Psychol. 2019;10:244.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Schmitz G, Kroeger D, Effenberg AO. A mobile sonification system for stroke rehabilitation. In: The 20th international conference on auditory display ICAD 2014. New York. 2014; 7.

  55. Schmitz G, Bergmann J, Effenberg AO, Krewer C, Hwang TH, Müller F. Movement sonification in stroke rehabilitation. Front Neurol. 2018;9:389.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Schnell N, Röbel A, Schwarz D, Peeters G, Borghesi R. MuBu and friends—Assembling tools for content based real-time interactive audio processing in Max/MSP. In: Proceedings of the International Computer Music Conference (ICMC), 2009.

  57. Scholz DS, Rhode S, Großbach M, Rollnik J, Altenmüller E. Moving with music for stroke rehabilitation: a sonification feasibility study: musical sonification in stroke rehabilitation. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2015;1337(1):69–76.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Scholz DS, Rohde S, Nikmaram N, Brückner HP, Großbach M, Rollnik JD, Altenmüller EO. Sonification of arm movements in stroke rehabilitation—a novel approach in neurologic music therapy. Front Neurol. 2016. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3389/fneur.2016.00106.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Sethi A, Ting J, Allen M, Clark W, Weber D. Advances in motion and electromyography based wearable technology for upper extremity function rehabilitation: a review. J Hand Ther. 2020. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.jht.2019.12.021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Sigrist R, Rauter G, Riener R, Wolf P. Augmented visual, auditory, haptic, and multimodal feedback in motor learning: a review. Psychon Bull Rev. 2013;20(1):21–53.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Sihvonen AJ, Särkämö T, Leo V, Tervaniemi M, Altenmüller E, Soinila S. Music-based interventions in neurological rehabilitation. Lancet Neurol. 2017;16(8):648–60.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Tajadura-Jiménez A. Action sounds modulate arm reaching movements. Front Psychol. 2016;7:14.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Véron-Delor L, Pinto S, Eusebio A, Azulay JP, Witjas T, Velay JL, et al. Musical sonification improves motor control in Parkinson’s disease: a proof of concept with handwriting. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2019;1465:132.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Vickers P, Hogg B, Worrall D. Aesthetics of sonification: taking the subject-position. In: Wöllner C, editor. Body sound and space in music and beyond: multimodal explorations. Abingdon: Routledge; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Wang L, Peng JL, Xiang W, Huang YJ, Chen AL. Effects of rhythmic auditory stimulation on motor function and balance ability in stroke: a systematic review and meta-analysis of clinical randomized controlled studies. Front Neurosci. 2022. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3389/fnins.2022.1043575.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Winstein CJ, Stein J, Arena R, Bates B, Cherney LR, Cramer SC, Deruyter F, Eng JJ, Fisher B, Harvey RL, Lang CE, MacKay-Lyons M, Ottenbacher KJ, Pugh S, Reeves MJ, Richards LG, Stiers W, Zorowitz RD. Guidelines for adult stroke rehabilitation and recovery. Stroke. 2016;47(6):98–169.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Yoo GE, Kim SJ. Rhythmic auditory cueing in motor rehabilitation for stroke patients: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Music Ther. 2016;53(2):149–77.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Zhang Y, Cai J, Zhang Y, Ren T, Zhao M, Zhao Q. Improvement in stroke-induced motor dysfunction by music-supported therapy: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sci Rep. 2016;6(1):38521.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to express their gratitude to all the participants and to the staff of the department of rehabilitation of the Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, especially to the occupational therapist team.

Funding

This study was supported by Institut Universitaire d’Ingénierie en Santé (I. Peyre’s PhD scholarship from IUIS, Sorbonne Université), the Labex-SMART (ANR-11-IDEX-0004-02) and from the Element project (ANR-18-CE33-0002).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

IP, ARB, MS, VMP and FB conceptualized the study and developed the protocol. IP and PP-D have selected the patients and healthy participants, and performed the experiments. IP, ARB, FB, BC and AG analyzed the data and wrote the draft of the manuscript. AG performed the statistics. All authors participated in finalizing the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Frédéric Bevilacqua.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the appropriate ethic committee prior to the beginning of the study (CERES: 2016-55, University of Paris-Descartes IRB: 20165500001072). All participants signed an informed consent.

Consent for publication:

All the authors approved the publication of the article.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1:

 S1. Guide of semi-structured interview. S2. Sound Spectrogram. S3. Descriptive Data of Participant’s Profiles. S4. Amusia Scores. S5. No Sound Serie. S6. Sonification Categories results. S7. Sound condition results.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Peyre, I., Roby-Brami, A., Segalen, M. et al. Effect of sonification types in upper-limb movement: a quantitative and qualitative study in hemiparetic and healthy participants. J NeuroEngineering Rehabil 20, 136 (2023). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12984-023-01248-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12984-023-01248-y

Keywords