Skip to main content

Adaptive conjunctive cognitive training (ACCT) in virtual reality for chronic stroke patients: a randomized controlled pilot trial

Abstract

Background

Current evidence for the effectiveness of post-stroke cognitive rehabilitation is weak, possibly due to two reasons. First, patients typically express cognitive deficits in several domains. Therapies focusing on specific cognitive deficits might not address their interrelated neurological nature. Second, co-occurring psychological problems are often neglected or not diagnosed, although post-stroke depression is common and related to cognitive deficits. This pilot trial aims to test a rehabilitation program in virtual reality that trains various cognitive domains in conjunction, by adapting to the patient’s disability and while investigating the influence of comorbidities.

Methods

Thirty community-dwelling stroke patients at the chronic stage and suffering from cognitive impairment performed 30 min of daily training for 6 weeks. The experimental group followed, so called, adaptive conjunctive cognitive training (ACCT) using RGS, whereas the control group solved standard cognitive tasks at home for an equivalent amount of time. A comprehensive test battery covering executive function, spatial awareness, attention, and memory as well as independence, depression, and motor impairment was applied at baseline, at 6 weeks and 18-weeks follow-up.

Results

At baseline, 75% of our sample had an impairment in more than one cognitive domain. The experimental group showed improvements in attention (\( {\chi}_F^2 \) (2) = 9.57, p < .01), spatial awareness (\( {\chi}_F^2 \) (2) = 11.23, p < .01) and generalized cognitive functioning (\( {\chi}_F^2 \) (2) = 15.5, p < .001). No significant change was seen in the executive function and memory domain. For the control group, no significant change over time was found. Further, they worsened in their depression level after treatment (T = 45, r = .72, p < .01) but returned to baseline at follow-up. The experimental group displayed a lower level of depression than the control group after treatment (Ws = 81.5, z = − 2.76, r = − .60, p < .01) and (Ws = 92, z = − 2.03, r = − .44, p < .05).

Conclusions

ACCT positively influences attention and spatial awareness, as well as depressive mood in chronic stroke patients.

Trial registration

The trial was registered prospectively at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT02816008) on June 21, 2016.

Background

Cognitive impairments are common after stroke, with incident rates up to 78% [1]. Patients with mild cognitive impairment are at risk for developing dementia [2]. Cognitive deficits correlate with poor functional outcomes and increased risk of dependence [3], have negative effects on the patient’s quality of life [4], and alter the patient’s ability to socialize [5]. However, the current clinical practice seems to lack methods that specifically address cognitive sequelae. According to a meta-analysis that aimed at proposing recommendations for new clinical standards, currently available treatments that are used as control conditions are conventional therapies like physical therapy or occupational therapy, pseudo treatments like mental or social stimulation without therapeutic intent, as well as psychosocial interventions like psychotherapy or emotional support for individuals or groups [6]. Besides, it has been shown that cognitively impaired patients participate less in rehabilitation activities, which potentially contributes to the poorer functional outcome they display [7]. Finding effective cognitive rehabilitation methods that can be incorporated in clinical practice is therefore crucial. Numerous methods to improve cognitive deficits, for instance, specifically attention [8], memory [9], executive function [10], or spatial abilities [11], have been proposed. However, the results show mixed efficacies. A meta-analysis on the impact of attentional treatments showed an effect on divided attention in the short-term, but found no evidence for persisting effects on other attentional domains, global attention, or functional outcomes [12]. Similarly, a meta-review that investigated the effect of memory rehabilitation found that training might benefit subjective reports of memory in the short term, but shows no effect in the long term, on objective memory measures, mood, functional abilities or quality of life [13]. Ultimately, a meta-analysis over 6 Cochrane reviews shows insufficient research evidence or evidence of insufficient quality to support any recommendation for cognitive stroke rehabilitation [14]. Besides methodological issues, one limitation of existing methods could be that they focus on one deficit only, ignoring that patients typically express deficits in multiple cognitive domains [1, 2]. A study on a large sample of heterogeneous stroke patients which aimed at linking lesions to cognitive deficits found that a given lesion location leads to cognitive impairments in several domains [15]. This emphasizes that cognitive functions rely on a network of brain regions. A lesion in one of those regions might cause a disturbance to the network, which leads to a multitude of symptoms. This is further supported by studies that revealed that pathological changes in brain structures are related to the occurrence of various cognitive deficits and symptoms for instance, in Alzheimer’s disease [16] or spatial neglect [17]. Moreover, the presence of multiple cognitive deficits seems to be a marker in patients that are at risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease later in life [18]. To what extent rehabilitation could potentially drive structural or functional changes to alleviate the symptoms of stroke is still under debate [19, 20]. Nevertheless, rehabilitation methods have to aid the patient in obtaining enough functionality to independently perform instrumental activities of daily living, be it through restoration of function or compensation. With this in mind, focusing on training a single cognitive skill might not be efficient because many daily tasks or jobs require several cognitive abilities for their execution [21]. For instance, most patients would like to be mobile and drive a car again after their stroke. Driving requires the individual to use selective attention to deal with the traffic, traffic signs and distractions, to be cognitively flexible to react to changing situations on the road, to visually scan the mirrors at the front, at the side, and in the back, to have a visual field that includes the sidewalks and to perform all of this while steering the car effectively in real-time [22]. Consequently, rehabilitation methods that address one specific cognitive ability only do not address the requirements of performing the activities of daily living and might not stimulate and train the underlying brain processes adequately. If a stroke leads to impairments in various cognitive domains, then these domains should be treated together to benefit a patient’s performance in everyday life.

To address the challenge of simultaneously training various cognitive abilities in an individualized manner, we revert to interactive technologies, in particular to the coupling of motion capture technology with virtual reality (VR). VR-based systems have shown to be at least as effective as conventional therapies for physical rehabilitation, such as for the recovery of upper limb movements [23,24,25] or gait and balance [26]. Contrarily, meta-analyses investigating the use of VR for stroke rehabilitation were either not able to analyze the effect of training on cognitive function [25] or only found a preliminary positive effect [24] due to insufficient randomized controlled trials. Besides, computer-based interventions for cognitive rehabilitation are currently only recommended as a practice option when supervised by a therapist [27]. The positive effect of VR for physical recovery, however, is only confirmed for those systems that incorporate distinct neuroscientific and psychological principles that underlie learning and recovery [23, 28]. It appears that cognitive rehabilitation methods can also include principles of learning, like repetitive practice, increasing difficulty or complexity and providing feedback through auditory or verbal cues [29, 30]. However, it seems that these principles are either not explicitly declared in the interventions, or the field still needs to evaluate the exact mechanisms behind cognitive rehabilitation that would positively alter cognitive function and behaviour [14]. This leads to the paradoxical situation, that although many cognitive rehabilitation protocols rely on technology (18 out of 44 studies in the meta-analyses mentioned here [10,11,12, 31]), VR appears to be rarely used in cognitive rehabilitation (4 studies in [29]). More specifically, certain principles of neurorehabilitation can be better implemented in virtual than in physical reality. For instance, a recent study has shown that the intention compatible enhancement of movement is beneficial in counteracting learned non-use [32]. This enhancement is only possible when the properties of visual feedback are manipulated beyond the properties of the physical world. There are indications that such enhanced feedback can be used in cognitive rehabilitation too. Some rehabilitation methods for reducing spatial neglect use VR to recreate realistic scenarios (e.g., crossroads) that allow the patients to train attentional abilities in an ecologically valid but safe environment [29]. Augmented visual or auditory feedback provides them with a more enriched and controlled learning situation than reality would be able to offer [33]. The VR system used in the current study combines specifically two principles of neurorehabilitation: increasing and individualizing difficulty as well as embodied first-person practice [28]. The principle of increasing difficulty is grounded on the finding that learning is maximal if a task is individualized to the subject and provides training at an optimal challenge level [34, 35]. This principle was also advanced as being beneficial for cognitive rehabilitation [36]. A study that provided computerized working memory training which increased the difficulty level of each training task automatically to the patient’s working memory capacity found a significant improvement in trained and untrained working memory tasks [9], which is similar to another study where the difficulty adapted as a function of individual performance and where feedback was provided through scores and verbal encouragement [37]. Indeed, in VR, we can create tasks that require the patient to use abilities from various cognitive domains to achieve a given goal [38]. Algorithms can learn from the patient’s performance and adapt the difficulty of the task gradually and automatically to identify the current ability level of the patient and to challenge it appropriately [39], potentially allowing a heterogeneous group of cognitively impaired individuals to train in a consistent rehabilitation regime. The principle of embodied practice relies on the insights gained from the studies of action observation [40]. It is also the primary rationale behind the Rehabilitation Gaming System (RGS) [41, 42], a VR rehabilitation tool on which the development of the training program presented here is based. RGS promotes functional recovery at all stages post-stroke [43] and cortical reorganization [44] through an integrated approach that combines action execution and observation [45] with goal-oriented integrated tasks. In RGS the patient controls an avatar on a computer screen and observes the avatar’s movement from a first-person perspective. This embodied training could benefit cognitive rehabilitation too, as motor and cognitive skills training contributes to activity changes in common brain regions [46]. Indeed, earlier theoretical work has shown that we can also think of the motor system as forming an integral part of cognitive control systems [47, 48]. Besides delivering individualized, embodied and immersive training, using a VR-based system might also promote motivation through presenting complex goal-oriented tasks combined with gamification [49]. Patients identified the lack of motivation as one of the factors preventing them from completing post-stroke exercise programs [50]. Lack of adherence appears to be a known issue in cognitive rehabilitation as well [51]. However, the exact relationship between adherence and motivation as well as the factors which in turn define and affect internal states need to be investigated. Ultimately, VR-based systems are apt to increase training time and intensity and can extend the training to the patient’s home after discharge from the hospital [44], as they operate in an automated fashion, require less personnel, and are more cost-effective than traditional rehabilitation methods [52]. It is, therefore, worthwhile to investigate the effectiveness of science and evidence-based VR systems for cognitive recovery as they can overcome current limitations in cognitive rehabilitation, such as labour-intensiveness, isolated treatment of cognitive deficits and missing knowledge of the active ingredients in treatments [14].

Another issue in cognitive rehabilitation is that co-occurring post-stroke depression is often not detected [53]. However, depression is common after stroke—although incident rates can vary substantially between studies, pooled frequency is estimated to be at 31% [54]. Patients with post-stroke depression show lower cognitive functioning as well as a higher dependency in activities of daily living, more severe impairments, and handicap than non-depressed patients [55]. Poor performance in neuropsychological tests, therefore, can be attributed not only to stroke, age [56] and the inefficacy of cognitive training but also mood disorders. On the other hand, cognitive rehabilitation can influence depressive mood positively, as shown in patients with mild cognitive impairment [57]. Thus, the presence of depression should be measured in cognitively impaired patients, and its interaction with cognitive functioning and cognitive rehabilitation should be investigated when patients with cognitive deficits are treated.

Here, we propose and test a novel method for the conjunctive training of cognitive abilities from multiple cognitive domains. We developed integrated cognitive rehabilitation scenarios in VR to address deficits in memory, attention, spatial awareness, and executive function in combination and in a task- and goal-oriented manner. This proposal reflects the fundamental consideration that specific cognitive abilities are constituent aspects of cognition rather than isolated domains or, in other words, processes that are critically linked in the overall architecture of the brain [58]. The implementation of these scenarios includes a mechanism that adapts the difficulty automatically to the patient’s capabilities using machine learning techniques [59], thereby addressing unique profiles of impairments and skills in a heterogeneous group of stroke patients. The algorithm adapts several task parameters, which reflect cognitive abilities, to the performance of the patient and hence adjusts the task’s difficulty automatically. The task parameters fitting the user’s performance provide a user-specific model. The development of the adaptive conjunctive cognitive training (ACCT) program studied here is based on the existing VR rehabilitation tool RGS, which provides a task-oriented and gamified training from a first-person embodied perspective through an avatar immersed in multi-modal task environments [41, 42]. This explorative pilot study aims to identify potential effects and challenges in anticipation of a larger trial. We compare the ACCT intervention against a control group that performs a standard at-home cognitive rehabilitation program. We hypothesize that the training scenarios can adapt the difficulty to the individual cognitive impairment level of each patient, equalizing performance differences. Further, we expect to see that the ACCT intervention positively influences the patient’s impairment level in the four cognitive domains addressed. Knowing that observed effects could be potentially modulated by post-stroke depression, we also analyze in a subgroup whether depression negatively influences cognitive functioning and can be positively modulated by the ACCT intervention.

Methods

Study design and patients

We conducted a randomized controlled pilot trial with an intended allocation ratio of 1:1, which was approved by the local Ethical Committee at Parc de Salut Mar and registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT02816008). Recruitment and screening took place from August 2016 until August 2017 by the physicians from the neurological rehabilitation unit at Hospital d’Esperança in Barcelona. Potential participants were recruited and screened among the outpatients that visited the physicians for the yearly control at the hospital. This convenience sampling ensured a representative sample of community-dwelling chronic stroke patients. The inclusion criteria were as follows: a) cognitive impairment due to a first-ever stroke (Montreal Cognitive Assessment [60], MoCA < 26), b) no severe upper limb motor disability (Medical Research Council Scale for stroke assessment [61], MRC > 2), c) age between 45 and 75 years old and d) chronic state (more than 6 months after stroke but less than 10 years). The exclusion criteria were as follows: a) severe cognitive incapacity that prohibits the execution of the experiment, b) severe impairments like spasticity, communication disabilities (aphasia or apraxia) and perceptual or physical impairments that would interfere with the correct execution or understanding of the experiment, c) history of severe mental health problems that were present in the acute or subacute phase and d) presence of hemianopia. The reason for including patients with first-ever stroke only is that current literature is inconclusive whether a recurrent stroke enhances existing cognitive deficits or not [62, 63]. Inclusion and exclusion criteria, as well as stroke etiology, were checked by the physicians using standard clinical tools, the clinical history of the patient, and clinical appraisal. As there is no existing study from which estimations for our primary outcome measurements could have been obtained, the sample size had to be predicted instead of calculated through a power analysis. Based on our previous experiments that proved to be achievable with the resources and time available [32, 64], other trials with similar interventions [38, 65] and the doctor’s estimation of recruitment pace, a sample size of 30 participants was deemed adequate. The trial concluded when the sample size for a complete case analysis was reached. The Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) statement was used to report the trial.

Eligible patients that gave their written consent to participate were assessed by a neuropsychologist using the following tests: a neuropsychological test battery, additional clinical outcome scales and two VR assessments — at baseline (T0), after the intervention (T1) and at 3 months follow-up (T2). All assessments were conducted in the aforementioned order in one session, in the median 4 days before and 3 days after the intervention period. At baseline, the patients were randomized by the experimenter either into an experimental group (EG) or a control group (CG) using a custom-made computerized minimization procedure based on the open-source software OxMAR [66] to ensure balanced groups across the baseline characteristics (gender, age, days after stroke, MoCA, Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Barthel Index (BI) and Fugl-Meyer Assessment for the upper extremity (FM-UE)) and all the scores of the neuropsychological test battery (see section Outcome Measurements). Specifically, the measurements were stratified (dummy-coded) as follows: For the neuropsychological test battery and as well as the MoCA, MMSE, BI and FM-UE established cut-offs for the categories “no impairment”, “mild impairment”, “moderate impairment” or “severe impairment” were taken from normative data (see Additional file 1: Statistical Procedure), for age, the cut-off was set at 65, for days after stroke at 590 days and gender was categorized in male and female. First, a new patient would be stratified (dummy-coded with 0’s and 1’s) according to these cut-offs. Then the sums of the strati between the groups with the new patient added are compared. The patient is then either allocated to the group with the lower sum or if the sums are equal randomly allocated with a 50% chance for either group. The first four recruited patients were assigned using a computer-generated list of random numbers only known to the experimenter. Due to the nature of the intervention and personal resources, participants and the experimenter were not blind to the group allocation. The neuropsychologist was not informed about group allocation. However, since the assessments and the intervention took place in the same hospital it could not be prevented that some patients would cross path with the assessor. All patients underwent a six week long, daily training of 30 min, five times per week (Fig. 1a).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Experimental protocol and set-up. a The protocol lasted 18 weeks in total, 6 weeks of training, and 3-months follow-up period. b The set-up of the EG in the hospital consisted of a desktop computer, a Microsoft Kinect and two wristbands with reflective markers that are worn by the patient. A Tobii EyeTracker T120 tracked the eye movement of the patient during the training. The Kinect detects the reflective markers and transposes the movement of the patient's real arms onto the virtual arms of the avatar in the training scenarios. The patients are seated at a table, and the three training scenarios (c Complex Spheroids, d Star Constellations, and e Quality Controller) are shown on the screen always in the same order. Besides the automated adaptive difficulty mechanism and the embodied training, the system incorporates further principles of neurorehabilitation including the provision of multisensory feedback, feedback of results, variable and structured practice as well as promoting the use of the paretic limb. C Star Constellations, CG control group, D day, EG experimental group, Eval VR evaluation, Q Quality Controller, RGS Rehabilitation Gaming System, S Complex Spheroids

Experimental intervention

The EG played each day three cognitive training scenarios of 10 min each. The dose of training was estimated to be adequate based on the results from our previous studies in the motor domain [32] and the currently reported average intervention time [12]. The training was provided through the RGS set-up (Fig. 1b), a VR-based rehabilitation tool. After an initial introduction and explanation of the scenarios on the first day, the patients interacted independently with RGS. Every day the therapist on duty would place the patient in front of the motion capture sensor and the screen, log in to the system, and commence the intervention. Only a few patients required help with putting on and taking off the markers and to change between the training protocols. Apart from this, the therapist did neither assist during the intervention, check adherence to the goal of the task, nor provide any feedback to the patient. The therapist was, however, allowed to help when technical issues or computer problems arose. The data generated through the interaction with the system was automatically stored in a remote secured database at the experimenter’s institution. RGS has been validated in several clinical studies to be effective in functional motor recovery [32, 44, 64, 67]. With this pilot study, we extended the RGS framework of embodied training, where the patient controls a virtual avatar on a computer screen, with conjunctive cognitive training scenarios which we call ACCT. Besides, RGS incorporates an automated mechanism that adapts the difficulty of the training to each patient’s ability [59]. It is thought that training efficiency and engagement is maximal if the challenge level is optimal regarding performance, perceived difficulty and fatigue, e.g., a person learns maximally if the experienced difficulty is neither too low nor too high [68]. To maintain the perceived challenge within a scenario at a consistent level, the algorithm used here adapts task parameters (for instance the speed of moving objects, or the number of items that need to be memorized) which influence the actual difficulty based on the patient’s ongoing performance in the task [59]. Thus, when the patient is reaching a high level of performance, the algorithm makes the task more difficult, while when the patient’s performance drops, the algorithm makes the task easier. The task parameters were selected to train skills that underlie the cognitive domains investigated here, and based on existing literature on recommendations for effective cognitive rehabilitation. The skills and task parameters were combined into two training scenarios in order to provide the patients with multidomain exercises. The training scenarios and their task parameters are explained hereinafter. Performance is calculated as the relative success rate, e.g. the number of successful attempts over a given number of trials and the algorithm’s objective is to maintain it around 70–80%. In this study, we assume that the levels of the task parameters reflect the individual cognitive impairment levels. The tasks are briefly explained in the following paragraphs. Detailed information can be found in Additional file 1: Experimental Intervention and Additional file 2: Movie S1.

The Complex Spheroids scenario aims at training basic attention and memory ability without an automated adaptation of difficulty (Fig. 1c). It requires the patient to intercept approaching colored spheres by following a predefined sequence indicated at the top right corner of the screen.

In the Star Constellations scenario (Fig. 1d), a visuospatial short-term memory task [37], the patients must remember a given subset of stars in a constellation and reproduce them after a delay period. The difficulty level of four task parameters is adapted in this scenario: 1) The complexity and spatial extension of the constellations (seven levels) aim to train spatial attention and spatial memory. This parameter addresses the recommendation to offer a unique sequence of stimuli in each trial during working memory training [37], to progress from simpler to more complex tasks in executive function training [10] and to train the ability to detect and deploy attention to all sides of space [12]. 2) and 3) The number of stars in a subset and the time interval between their appearance should aid the training of working memory [37]. 4) The length of the delay period progressively challenges memory delayed recall. This parameter aids the training of internal strategies (visual imagery) which are recommended for memory training [6]. The countdown of the delay period serves as a non-spatial alerting intervention to train sustained attention [19].

In the Quality Controller scenario (Fig. 1e), patients are presented with two tasks concurrently. In the right workspace, doughnuts must be taken out of a fryer when their cooking time ends. In the left workspace, the patient must detect defective candies on a conveyor belt. The difficulty level of five task parameters is adapted in this scenario: 1) and 2) The speed of the conveyor belt, and the interval between appearing candies aim to train alertness. These parameters address speed-of-processing training that fosters visual search skills to identify and locate visual information quickly and in a divided-attention format [69]. 3) The ratio between defective and good candies is thought to promote selective and sustained attention. This parameter addresses the ability to focus on specific stimuli while ignoring irrelevant ones in attention training [12]. 4) The baking time of the doughnuts should train the ability to inhibit prepotent responses in executive function training [10]. 5) The time given to take the doughnuts out of the fryer should aid the training of initiation of behaviours in executive function training [10]. The alarm clock that signals when the doughnuts are ready should foster readiness to respond and, therefore, alertness and arousal [12]. The patient has to take care of the two spatially distributed tasks simultaneously; therefore, training divided attention ability, which is essential for multitasking and spatial attention [12]. The scenario should address bottom-up stimulus-driven alerting in spatial neglect [51] by promoting visual search, which improves voluntary exploration of the contralesional space [29]. It further addresses problem-solving and strategy formation techniques required in executive function [6, 10].

Control intervention

The CG received from the experimenter at the hospital a folder with 30 individual cognitive tasks that had to be completed at home (e.g., crosswords, spot the ten differences, draw complex figures reversed, or complete sentences) during 30 min at each workday. The tasks were selected by the neuropsychologist to overlap with the cognitive abilities essential in the experimental tasks (spatial awareness, attention, memory, executive ability) and to be representative of what would be generally suggested to community-dwelling patients for at-home training. The adherence to the control intervention was not monitored during the experiment. The patients were asked to write down the date and the time spent on each task and return the folder after 6 weeks. After the treatment, the patient would return the folder to the experimenter, who checked that the exercises were completed and asked the patients whether they had any difficulties fulfilling their task.

Outcome measures

The primary outcome measurements were four averaged standardized composite scores (ASCS) for attention, memory, executive function, and spatial awareness calculated from the neuropsychological test battery. The neuropsychological test battery was compiled by the neuropsychologist and covered the four cognitive domains. For attention, we chose the Corsi Block Tapping Test Forward (Corsi F) [70], the Trail Making Test A (TMT A) [71], and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition (WAIS) [72] Digit Span Forward (WAIS F). For memory, we selected the Corsi Block Tapping Test Backward (Corsi B) [70], the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test Immediate (RAVLT I) and Delayed Recall (RAVLT D) [73], and the WAIS Digit Span Backward (WAIS B). Executive function was covered by the TMT B, the WAIS Digit Symbol Coding (WAIS C) and the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) [74], and lastly, spatial awareness consisted of the Star Cancellation Test (Star) [75]. The standard scoring and Spanish test versions were used. Secondary outcomes were clinical scales that allowed us to check for additional effects of the treatment and consisted of the MoCA [60], the BI [76], the FM-UE [77], the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D) [78] and the MMSE [79]. The HAM-D outcome is only available for 21 subjects, as it was added after the first analysis of data [80]. Although patients with a history of severe mental health problems should have been excluded by our exclusion criteria, we suspected that mood might influence the results. In addition, the protocol included two VR assessments that will be analyzed in separate reports.

Statistical procedure

Since normality testing (Lilliefors test of normality) pointed out that most of our data except HAM-D were not normally distributed, we used non-parametric testing. Baseline characteristics and outcome measures were compared between groups using Wilcoxon’s rank-sum test (Ws) for interval and ordinal variables, and Pearson’s chi-square test (χ2) for nominal variables. Spearman’s correlation was used to assess how well the task parameters of each training scenario (median after 1 week of training) correlated with the neuropsychological test battery at baseline. For the primary outcomes, the individual test scores for each cognitive assessment were converted into standardized z-scores, using the mean and standard deviation (SD) of normative age-adjusted data. By averaging the z-scores, the ASCS for each domain were obtained. To obtain a measurement of generalized cognitive functioning, we took the median of the patient’s ASCS within each domain. Each patient’s ASCS per domain was stratified according to its SD from the normative mean to obtain the impairment level in each domain. The correlation within ASCS was evaluated using Spearman’s correlation. First, a within-group analysis was performed, evaluating the changes of ASCS scores and secondary outcomes over time across the three assessment points of the study (baseline T0, after treatment T1 and follow-up T2) using the Friedman’s ANOVA test statistic (\( {\chi}_F^2 \)). Then a post hoc analysis was performed using Wilcoxon’s sign rank test (T) comparing the scores after treatment and at follow-up with baseline. For the between-group analysis, the improvement after treatment (T1 – T0) and at follow-up (T2 – T0) was compared between EG and CG using the Wilcoxon’s rank-sum test (Ws). A complete case analysis and a last observation carried forward analysis were performed to deal with missing data. Significant results were only accepted when confirmed by both analyses. For the depression subgroup analysis, the improvement in ASCS was evaluated with a linear regression, in addition to the within- and between-group analysis. A detailed description of the statistical procedure can be found in the supplementary material (see Additional file 1: Statistical Procedure).

Results

We approached 59 chronic stroke patients, of which 47 agreed to participate and were assessed for eligibility (CONSORT flow diagram Fig. 2). Thirty-eight eligible individuals were assessed at baseline and randomized into EG (n = 19) and CG (n = 19). Their baseline characteristics can be found in Table 1. There were no differences between the groups in their baseline characteristics or in any of their baseline primary or secondary outcome measures. Three patients (CG = 2, EG = 1) withdrew after randomization. Thirty-five patients (EG = 18, CG = 17) completed the 6 weeks intervention program. In the CG, one patient was lost at post-assessment and two at follow-up. In EG, two patients were lost at post-assessment, resulting in 30 valid cases (EG = 16, CG = 14). Except for one patient that was able to complete only nine tasks, all the patients in the CG did complete all the paper and pencil tasks. However, only two patients noted down the time they spent on each task. Based on their reports, they spent between 20 to 30 min on each task, except for a few tasks they were able to finish in 5 minutes, and that should be replaced in the larger trial.

Fig. 2
figure 2

CONSORT flow diagram. CG control group, EG experimental group, MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MRC Medical Research Council Scale for stroke assessment

Table 1 Patient characteristics and secondary outcome measurements at baseline

Most of the patients showed an impairment in all four domains at baseline (Fig. 3a). Only five patients showed an impairment in a single domain, whereas two patients were better as the normative mean in all domains. Every domain contains a spread across all impairment levels (Fig. 3b). The Spearman’s correlation revealed that the ASCS of attention, memory and executive function, but not spatial awareness, of all patients together correlated significantly at baseline, after treatment, and at follow-up (Fig. 3c).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Impairment distribution and correlation at baseline. a Distribution of the number of domains impaired. b Distribution of severity per domain. c The ASCS for attention, memory, and executive function positively correlate at baseline, after treatment and follow-up. ASCS for spatial awareness seems to correlate weakly at baseline with the other domains, but this correlation is not visible after treatment and at follow-up. Significant p-values are indicated as * p-value < .05, ** p-value < .01, *** p-value < .001 and the color scale represents the correlation coefficient (Spearman’s r). ASCS averaged standardized composite score, ATB attention ASCS at baseline, ATF attention ASCS at follow-up, ATT attention ASCS after treatment, EF executive function, EFB executive function ASCS at baseline, EFF executive function ASCS at follow-up, EFT executive function ASCS after treatment, MEB memory ASCS at baseline, MEF memory ASCS at follow-up, MET memory ASCS after treatment, SA spatial awareness, SAB spatial awareness ASCS at baseline, SAF spatial awareness after follow-up, SAT spatial awareness at follow-up

In Fig. 4, we show the correlations between the median task parameters of each scenario, which regulate the difficulty of the training, after the first week of intervention, and the neuropsychological test battery at baseline in EG (n = 16). The analysis revealed that in the Star Constellations scenario (Fig. 4a) the median number of stars that a patient was able to remember correlated well with the scores in TMT A (rs = −.57, p < .05), Corsi B (rs = .67, p < .01), TMT B (rs = −.69, p < .01) and WAIS C (rs = .69, p < .01). Similarly, the median delay period achieved correlated well with the scores in TMT A (rs = −.56, p < .05) and Corsi B (rs = .68, p < .01), and moderately with WAIS C (rs = .46, p = .07). In addition, it correlated with Corsi F (rs = .54, p < .05) and WAIS B (rs = −.56, p < .05). Moreover, there was a correlation between the median constellation complexity level and WAIS C (rs = .59, p < .05). For the Quality Controller scenario (Fig. 4b) several correlations between task parameters and neuropsychological test battery scores have been found as well. The median speed of the conveyor belt and the ratio between good and defective candy correlated well with Corsi F (rs = .53, p < .05 and rs = .65, p < .01), TMT A (rs = −.61, p < .05 and rs = −.69, p < .01), RAVLT I (rs = .53, p < .05 and rs = .57, p < .05), TMT B (rs = −.46, p = .07 and rs = −.62, p < .05) and Star (rs = .65, p < .01 and rs = .75, p < .001). On the other hand, the median baking time and the median time to take out the doughnuts correlated with TMT A (rs = .54, p < .05 and rs = .46, p = .07), RAVLT I (rs = −.53, p < .05 and rs = −.45, p = .08), WAIS B (rs = −.60, p < .05 and rs = −.58, p < .05), FAB (rs = −.70, p < .01 and rs = −.53, p < .05), TMT B (rs = .65, p < .01 and rs = .53, p < .05), WAIS C (rs = −.47, p = .06, and rs = −.53, p < .05) and Star (rs = −.47, p = .07 and rs = −.58, p < .05).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Correlations between the task parameters of the training scenarios and the neuropsychological test battery. a The task parameters of the Star Constellations scenario are the constellation complexity level (category), the number of stars in the subset (number of stars), the time interval between their appearance (interval) and the length of the delay period (delay period). b The task parameters of the Quality Controller scenario are the speed of the conveyor belt (speed), the time interval between the appearance of the candies (interval), the ratio between defective and good candies (ratio), the baking time of the doughnuts (baking time) and the time given to take the doughnuts out of the fryer (taking time). The number represents the p-value and the color scale represents the correlation coefficient (Spearman’s r). Corsi B Corsi Block Tapping Test Backward, Corsi F Corsi Block Tapping Test Forward, EF executive function domain, FAB Frontal Assessment Battery, RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, RAVLT I RAVLT Immediate, RAVLT D Delayed Recall, Star Star Cancellation Test, SA spatial awareness domain, TMT A Trail Making Test A, TMT B Trail Making Test B, WAIS Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition, WAIS B WAIS Backward, WAIS C WAIS Digit Symbol Coding, WAIS F WAIS Digit Span Forward

The algorithm adapted the task parameters well to the individual impairment level in EG (n = 16), ensuring a stable success rate while training (Fig. 5). For instance, in the Star Constellations scenario, stratifying patients according to their impairment level in the spatial awareness domain at baseline revealed that more severe patients achieved lower difficulty levels than less impaired ones (Fig. 5a). Throughout the training, however, the achieved difficulty level seemed to increase across all severity levels. Although the task parameter levels differed for each patient, the success rate remained stable at around 70% (Fig. 5b). The same pattern can be observed in the Quality Controller scenario (Fig. 5c and d). Here, however, the achieved task parameter might not have been challenging enough for non-impaired patients as their performance was around 90% (Fig. 5d).

Fig. 5
figure 5

The relationship between impairment level, difficulty achievement, and performance (success rate) within training scenarios. a Difficulty achievement in the Star Constellations scenario and c in the Quality Controller scenario. The task parameter in Star Constellations is the constellation complexity level, and in Quality Controller the speed of the conveyor belt. Success rate (number of successful attempts over all possible trials in percentage) for Star Constellations (b) and Quality Controller (d). Possible trials in Star Constellations are the total number of constellations shown in a session. In Quality Controller success rate represents the true positives of all defective candies in a session. Solid line and error bars represent median and median absolute deviation per 10 sessions (two weeks), data points represent individual patients stratified according to their impairment level in spatial awareness domain for Star Constellations and executive function domain for Quality Controller at baseline: severe (red cross), moderate (violet circle), mild (blue triangle) and no impairment (green square). EF executive function domain, MAD median absolute deviation, SA spatial awareness domain

In Table 2, we show the descriptive data of the ASCS for every domain at baseline (T0), after treatment (T1) and at follow-up (T2) as well as the p-values of the within-group analysis for the complete cases (EG = 16, CG = 14). The data for the last observation carried forward analysis (EG = 19, CG = 19) can be found in the Additional file 1: Table S1. We found a significant change in ASCS over time for the EG in the attention domain (\( {\chi}_F^2 \) (2) = 9.57, p < .01), in the spatial awareness domain (\( {\chi}_F^2 \) (2) = 11.23, p < .01) and in the generalized cognitive functioning (\( {\chi}_F^2 \) (2) = 14.00, p < .001) in the complete case analysis (Fig. 6a-c), which was confirmed by the last observation carried forward analysis. In the attention domain, the post hoc analysis revealed significantly higher scores at T2 (T = 84.5, r = .48, p < .01) as compared to baseline. In the spatial awareness domain, the post hoc analysis revealed significant higher scores at T1 (T = 47, r = .35, p < .05) and at T2 (T = 63, r = .47, p < .01) as compared to baseline. In the generalized cognitive functioning, the post hoc analysis indicated significant higher scores at T1 (T = 130, r = .59, p < .01) and at T2 (T = 123, r = .52, p < .01) as compared to baseline. For the CG, no significant change over time was found, although the memory domain yielded significantly higher scores at T1 (T = 86, r = .56, p < .05) that was confirmed by the last observation carried forward analysis. No significant results for either group were found in the executive function domain. Neither we found significant differences between the groups in the complete case analysis that would have been confirmed in the last observation carried forward analysis (Table 3). The descriptive statistics for every test in the neuropsychological test battery can be found in Additional file 1: Table S2.

Table 2 ASCS at baseline (T0), after treatment (T1) and follow-up (T2) and the p-values for the within-group analysis of the change over time for the complete case analysis
Fig. 6
figure 6

Main findings in ASCS scores and subgroup analysis. Change in (a) attention ASCS, (b) spatial awareness ASCS, (c) generalized cognitive functioning ASCS, and (d) depression (HAM-D) from baseline to after treatment (T1-T0) and to follow-up (T2-T0) for the experimental group (EG, green) and control group (CG, red). The individual data for each subject is indicated with dots. Negative numbers in HAM-D mean improvement (less depression). The ASCS scores change for memory, and executive function can be found in Additional file 1: Figure S1. MAD median absolute deviation, SE standard error of the mean

Table 3 Between-group analysis of baseline ASCS (T0) as well as improvement in ASCS after treatment (T1 – T0) and at follow-up (T2 – T0)

Additional file 1: Table S3 shows the results for the secondary outcomes. We found a significant change over time only in MMSE for CG (\( {\chi}_F^2 \) (2) = 7.14, p < .05). Post hoc analysis revealed a significant difference between T0 and T2 (T = 62.5, r = .72, p < .01). For the EG we found that FM-UE after treatment was significant different from baseline (T = 43, r = .61, p < .05) and that this improvement was also significant different from the improvement of the CG (Ws = 288.5, z = 2.22, r = .40, p < .05). No other significant results in the secondary outcomes were found (see Additional file 1: Figure S2).

In Table 4, we report the results of the within-group analysis for the depression subgroup analysis (EGD = 11, CGD = 10). The CGD shows a significant worsening in the HAM-D at T1 (T = 45, r = .72, p < .01) as compared to baseline. At T1, the depression level of the CGD was significantly higher in comparison to the EGD (Ws = 81.5, z = − 2.76, r = − .60, p < .01) and this difference remained significant at T2 (Ws = 92, z = − 2.03, r = − .44, p < .05), see Table 5 and Fig. 6d. We observed a significant effect of time for EGD in the attention domain (\( {\chi}_F^2 \) (2) = 10.82, p < .01) and in the generalized cognitive functioning domain (\( {\chi}_F^2 \) (2) = 9.8, p < .01). Post hoc analysis in the attention domain revealed a significant difference between T0 and T2 (T = 43.5, r = 0.53, p < .05) and between T1 and T2 (T = 40.5, r = .46, p < .05). Post hoc analysis in the generalized cognitive functioning domain showed a significant difference from T0 to T1 (T = 53, r = .57, p < .01) and from T0 to T2 (T = 53, r = .57, p < .01). In addition, we found a difference between the two groups improvement at T1 in generalized cognitive functioning (Ws = 151, r = .45, p < .05). For CGD, no change over time was found. These results are similar to what was found in the analysis of the whole study sample. On the other hand, we could only confirm a reduced influence of the level of depression on the performance on the neuropsychological test battery. Of the eleven tests included in our battery, three correlated with the HAM-D at baseline (Corsi F: r = − .69, p < .05, TMT A: r = .45, p < .05, TMT B: r = .47, p < .05). These correlations disappeared after the treatment and at follow-up.

Table 4 Depression subgroup analysis. HAM-D and ASCS at baseline (T0), after treatment (T1) and follow-up (T2) and the p-values for the within-group analysis of the change over time
Table 5 Between-group analysis in depression subgroup of baseline HAM-D and ASCS (T0) as well as improvement in HAM-D and ASCS after treatment (T1 – T0) and at follow-up (T2 – T0)

One patient in EGD showed a particularly large improvement of 13 points in HAM-D from T0 to T1. To check if this improvement influenced the results found, we performed the subgroup analysis without this patient. After excluding the patient, we observed that the difference between the groups at T2 loses significance as the p-value changes from .04 to .07. However, the EGD group continues to express lower depression levels at T2 (mean of 4.40) than the CGD (mean 6.30). The same patient also showed improvements in attention, memory, and spatial awareness. The exclusion of the patient in the analysis of the cognitive domains did not alter the results found, whether in the subgroup analysis nor in the analysis of the whole sample. We, therefore, did not deem this patient as an outlier that had to be excluded from the analysis.

Next, we wanted to see how the improvement in the cognitive domains influenced the improvement in depression level in our subgroup. We included the improvements in ASCS at T1 (T1 – T0) and T2 (T2 – T0) in a linear regression to estimate the respective depression improvement (Table 6). We found a marginally significant prediction power of improvement in attention ASCS (t(17) = − 1.99, p = .06) and a significant effect of improvement in memory ASCS (t(17) = − 2.35, p < .05) to predict the patient’s change in HAM-D from baseline to follow-up. These results indicate that improvement in the domains of attention and memory is positively correlated with improvement in depression .

Table 6 Results from a linear regression on improvement in depression

Discussion

In this randomized controlled pilot trial, we tested a novel rehabilitation program in VR that trains several cognitive domains in conjunction. Together with a few other clinical trials [38, 65], we are among the first in addressing the multidimensionality of cognitive impairment after stroke, by providing a VR-based cognitive training that adapts its difficulty optimally to the ability of the patient while providing an embodied training with rewarding feedback [81]. Our data set reveals interesting insights when a heterogeneous sample without a specific cognitive deficit is selected. Similar to prospective studies [1, 2], we see that patients show an impairment in more than one domain. The majority was impaired in all four domains. Also, the impairments in the attention, memory, and executive function domain, but not in the spatial awareness domain, are correlated and remain so over time. The rationale behind the training scenarios is that several cognitive skills can be trained together in a multidomain fashion. With the Star Constellations scenario, we intended to address visuospatial working memory and attentional skills. The correlations between the median task parameters achieved after the first week of training and the scores of the neuropsychological test battery at baseline appears to confirm this intention: TMT A, TMT B, and WAIS C are timed and require online visual tracking ability [82, 83], whereas Corsi F, Corsi B, and WAIS B require working memory skills [84, 85], which in the case of Corsi are paired with a visual component [82]. Besides, we found a correlation of the median constellation complexity level with WAIS C, a test that requires fast decoding of number-symbol combinations [82]. With the Quality Controller scenario, we intended to provide a speeded and distributed dual-task training. The correlations of four task parameters with TMT A and TMT B confirms a strong speed-of-processing and attentional switching component [85], whereas the correlation with Star refers to the visual components trained due to the spatially distributed task. The correlations between the timed task components (baking and taking out time) and the tests of the executive function domain supports the training of inhibition and initiation of responses [30] whereas the correlations with Corsi F, RAVLT I and WAIS B point additionally to a memory component inherent to the training [85,86,87]. We further demonstrated that our system successfully takes the individual impairment level into account and enables the patients to achieve similar success rates despite varying levels of impairment. The difference in the performance achieved between the two scenarios, especially by the non-impaired patients, might be due to a difference in difficulty between subsequent levels; i.e., in the Quality Controller scenario, the next difficulty level was too hard to achieve, so the patients remained on a lower level thus achieving higher success rates. This illustrates the importance of individualizing training through fine-graded difficulty levels to promote learning and rehabilitation but as well highlights the challenges of doing so [88, 89].

Regarding the four cognitive domains assessed, only the EG shows a significant change over time in attention and spatial awareness. We did not see any significant change in the ASCS over time in the CG, who did cognitive pencil and paper exercises at home. In addition, generalized cognitive functioning increased in EG from baseline to follow-up. We are aware that due to the small sample size in this pilot study and the multiple testing, these results could be spurious, and we can therefore not claim any rehabilitation effect. However, the effect found in generalized cognitive functioning seems to be robust because it includes all ASCS and cannot be driven by the improvement in attention alone. Further, a positive change of attention ASCS and generalized cognitive functioning is still present in the depression subgroup analysis. Interestingly the significant changes in EG were confirmed for the follow-up period, as demonstrated by the post hoc analysis at this time point. We could speculate that this delayed effect of training could mean that the patient incorporated what they learned during the training later in their daily activities, similar to what has been observed in cognitive strategy training [90].

Whether the significant changes in attention and spatial awareness ASCS are clinically relevant is difficult to evaluate, as there is no consensus in literature with regards to the clinically important difference (CID) in neuropsychological test batteries. CIDs reported in studies range from 0.5 SD [91] to 1 SD [92] up to 2 SD [93]. Applying a cut-off of 0.5 SD and 1 SD to our sample (see Additional file 1: Table S4 and S5) shows that still more patients in EG improve even above 1 SD from baseline, especially after follow-up. However, future studies should direct their efforts to find ways to standardize neuropsychological testing and establishing CIDs in well-powered clinical studies.

Regarding the secondary outcomes, the CG showed a significant change in the MMSE over time, with post hoc analysis revealing a significant difference between baseline and follow-up (see Additional file 1: Table S3). On the other hand, no change over time was observed in the MoCA for either group. Interestingly, according to MMSE, only one patient would have been classified as having a cognitive impairment at baseline. This finding is in line with literature, where it was observed that the MoCA is more sensitive to cognitive dysfunction than the MMSE [94]. Also, we used at each assessment point a different test variation of the MoCA, so that the patients never repeated the same exercises. The MMSE, however, is only available in one version, and some exercises resemble the cognitive pencil and paper exercises used in the CG, which might have helped them to succeed in this test.

We further found a significant but small group difference in the FM-UE improvement after treatment in favour of the EG (Additional file 1: Table S3). Although the experimental intervention includes a stronger motor component than the control intervention, motor training was not the focus of the study. Therefore, only patients with sufficient active movement and able to overcome gravity (MRC > 2) were included, although the tasks were accomplished by moving the arms only horizontally supported by a table’s surface. Also, the mean change is below the CID [95] and MDC [96], although four patients surpassed the CID threshold of 4.25 (see Additional file 1: Figure S2) at follow-up. However, in a general stroke population, motor and cognitive deficits likely co-occur [15], and cognitive deficits have a negative effect on functional outcome and independence [3]. It has been stressed out that rehabilitation should combine motor and cognitive training [81]. It would, therefore, be interesting to investigate the effect of the proposed training paradigm that already includes a motor component in patients with lower motor functionality. We believe that patients with more severe motor impairments could easily participate in the ACCT program since no movement against gravity is required, and the adaptive difficulty algorithm could ensure that the arrangement of the interactive elements in the training scenarios does not surpass the patient’s active range of motion. Besides, the ACCT program could be complemented with another adaptive mechanism that aids the completion of goal-oriented movements with the paretic arm through a visual manipulation [32].

Lastly, the subgroup analysis revealed that, compared to the EG, the CG expressed higher depression levels after the intervention. The groups remained significantly different at follow-up. We cannot exclude that the non-blinding of group allocation or that the control task that had to be done at home negatively influenced the depression level in the CG. However, we also see a trend for EG to reduce their depression level. This could be due to the alleviation of rumination, a known symptom of depression, which has been proposed by the attention restoration theory to occur when a patient successfully breaks away from routine physical and mental tasks and switches from an effortful, directed attention to an interest-driven one – both of which can be achieved by providing an adequate environment that is stimuli rich, coherently structured and allows for exploration [97]. The ACCT intervention in the hospital might provide such an environment, whereas the paper and pencil intervention at home does not. The subgroup analysis also replicated the sample’s improvement over time in attention ASCS of the complete case analysis. Improvement in attention and memory ASCS predicted depression improvement at follow-up; the more patients improved in attention and memory ASCS, the more they improved in depression. Notice, however, that the directionality of this relationship remains unclear. However, the intercept indicates that there seems to be a negative improvement in depression if no improvement in attention or memory is present. It is known that depression correlates with cognitive deficits, specifically in nonverbal problem solving, verbal and visual memory and attention, and psychomotor speed [55]. Potentially, the improvement in attention or memory through training resulted also in a reduction of depression levels in our sample. Alternatively, the training induced a change in mood, which resulted in cognitive improvement. This subgroup analysis is particularly interesting because, according to our exclusion criteria, patients with mental illness should not have passed the screening process. This result underlines the notion that mental problems often remain undiagnosed or are neglected when assessing the health status of the patient, despite the known impact of depressive mood on cognitive ability, independence, impairment, and handicap [55].

There are several limitations to this study. Firstly, this pilot comprises of a small sample size. More patients would be necessary to confirm the indicated results with adequate power. Also, a larger sample is necessary to check if specific cognitive aspects of the training scenarios influence groups of patients with similar deficit profiles differently. However, the number of neuropsychological tests performed was excessive for the sample size tested. Further, we are aware that the experimental intervention appears to be substantially different from the control intervention in terms of location (hospital versus home) and human interaction (therapist versus, possibly, caregiver). Although a control intervention in the hospital would appear appropriate on methodological grounds, our control condition represents the reality of community-dwelling stroke patients and is, therefore, closer to the “best available” treatment [36]. Besides, the EG did not receive more attention from a therapist than the CG. The patients at the hospital were independently completing their daily tasks, only receiving technical support from the therapist when needed and no performance feedback. However, it cannot be excluded that the different locations might have exposed the patients in the EG to a richer social environment and influenced our results. Hence future studies should test for the potential effect of location on cognition or depression and take it into account when designing their protocols. Further, we were not able to blind the patients and could only partly blind the outcome assessor. This is, unfortunately, a problem frequently encountered in studies evaluating VR-interventions. Nevertheless, we believe that our results support the growing evidence that recovery of cognitive functioning after stroke is possible. Since we were able to train stroke survivors with heterogeneity in cognitive impairment, it fuels the hope that rehabilitation approaches in VR that are grounded on neuroscientific principles of recovery could potentially address co-occurring symptoms even independent of disease or aetiology [98]. Future work should, therefore, test the proposed training paradigm in other patient groups with similar cognitive symptomatology.

Conclusions

Our stroke rehabilitation approach, called ACCT, was able to adapt the training to the individual cognitive deficit of the patients, and initial results indicated that the training reduced the impairment in two out of four cognitive domains. In addition, a positive change in the mental wellbeing of the patients was observed. This work, therefore, highlights the importance of addressing cognitive domains in conjunction as well as considering the psychological sequelae after a stroke incident.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ACCT:

Adaptive conjunctive cognitive training

ASCS:

Averaged standardized composite score

BI:

Barthel Index

CG:

Control group

CGD:

Control group in depression subgroup analysis

CID:

Clinically important difference

CONSORT:

Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials

Corsi B / Corsi F:

Corsi Block Tapping Test Backward / Forward

EG:

Experimental group

EGD:

Experimental group in depression subgroup analysis

FAB:

Frontal Assessment Battery

FM-UE:

Fugl-Meyer Assessment for the upper extremity

HAM-D:

Hamilton Depression Rating Scale

MDC:

Minimum detectable change

MMSE:

Mini-Mental State Examination

MoCA:

Montreal Cognitive Assessment

MRC:

Medical Research Council Scale for stroke assessment

RAVLT D / I:

Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test Delayed Recall / Immediate

RGS:

Rehabilitation Gaming System

SD:

Standard deviation

Star:

Star Cancellation Test

TMT A / B:

Trail Making Test A / B

VR:

Virtual reality

WAIS B / C / F:

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition Digit Span Backward / Digit Symbol Coding / Digit Span Forward

References

  1. Leśniak M, Bak T, Czepiel W, Seniów J, Członkowska A. Frequency and prognostic value of cognitive disorders in stroke patients. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;26(4):356–63. Available from: https://www.karger.com/Article/FullText/162262.

  2. Rasquin SMC, Lodder J, Ponds RWHM, Winkens I, Jolles J, Verhey FRJ. Cognitive functioning after stroke: a one-year follow-up study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2004;18(2):138–44. Available from: https://www.karger.com/Article/FullText/79193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Paolucci S, Antonucci G, Gialloreti E, Traballesi M, Lubich S, Pratesi L, et al. Predicting stroke inpatient rehabilitation outcome: the prominent role of neuropsychological disorders. Eur Neurol. 1996;36(6):385–90 Available from: http://www.karger.com/?doi=10.1159/000117298.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Park JH, Kim J, Bae H, Lee J, Lee J, Han M, et al. Impact of post-stroke cognitive impairment with no dementia on health-related quality of life. J Stroke. 2013;15(1):49–56. Available from: http://j-stroke.org/journal/view.php?doi=10.5853/jos.2013.15.1.49.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Mukherjee D, Levin RL, Heller W. The cognitive, emotional, and social Sequelae of stroke: psychological and ethical concerns in post-stroke adaptation. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2006;13(4):26–35 Available from: http://0-www-tandfonline-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/full/10.1310/tsr1304-26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Cicerone KD, Dahlberg C, Malec JF, Langenbahn DM, Felicetti T, Kneipp S, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the literature from 1998 through 2002. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2005;86(8):1681–92. Available from: https://0-linkinghub-elsevier-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/retrieve/pii/S0003999305003308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Skidmore ER, Whyte EM, Holm MB, Becker JT, Butters MA, Dew MA, et al. Cognitive and affective predictors of rehabilitation participation after stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2010;91(2):203–7 Available from: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.apmr.2009.10.026.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Barker-Collo SL, Feigin VL, Lawes CMM, Parag V, Senior H, Rodgers A. Reducing attention deficits after stroke using attention process training: a randomized controlled trial. Stroke. 2009;40(10):3293–8 Available from: http://stroke.ahajournals.org/cgi/doi/10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.558239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Lundqvist A, Grundström K, Samuelsson K, Rönnberg J. Computerized training of working memory in a group of patients suffering from acquired brain injury. Brain Inj. 2010;24(10):1173–83 Available from: http://0-www-tandfonline-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/full/10.3109/02699052.2010.498007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Chung CS, Pollock A, Campbell T, Durward BR, Hagen S. Cognitive rehabilitation for executive dysfunction in adults with stroke or other adult non-progressive acquired brain damage. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2013;(4) Available from: http://0-onlinelibrary-wiley-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/10.1002/14651858.CD008391/full.

  11. Bowen A, Lincoln N. Cognitive rehabilitation for spatial neglect following stroke. In: Bowen A, editor. Cochrane database of systematic reviews. Wiley: Chichester; 2007. Available from: http://0-doi-wiley-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/14651858.CD003586.pub2.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Loetscher T, Lincoln NB. Cognitive rehabilitation for attention deficits following stroke. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2013;(5) Available from: http://0-doi-wiley-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/14651858.CD002842.pub2.

  13. das Nair R, Cogger H, Worthington E. Cognitive rehabilitation for memory deficits after stroke. Stroke. 2017;48(2):1739. Available from: https://www.ahajournals.org/doi/10.1161/STROKEAHA.116.015377.

  14. Gillespie DC, Bowen A, Chung CS, Cockburn J, Knapp P, Pollock A. Rehabilitation for post-stroke cognitive impairment: an overview of recommendations arising from systematic reviews of current evidence. Clin Rehabil. 2015;29(2):120–8 Available from: http://0-www-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov.brum.beds.ac.uk/pubmed/24942480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Corbetta M, Ramsey L, Callejas A, Baldassarre A, Hacker CD, Siegel JS, et al. Common behavioral clusters and subcortical anatomy in stroke. Neuron. 2015;85(5):927–41 Available from: http://0-linkinghub-elsevier-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/retrieve/pii/S0896627315001427.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Perry RJ, Hodges JR. Attention and executive deficits in Alzheimer’s disease. A critical review. Brain. 1999;122(3):383–404. Available from: https://0-academic-oup-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/brain/article-lookup/doi/10.1093/brain/122.3.383.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Malhotra P, Mannan S, Driver J, Husain M. Impaired spatial working memory: one component of the visual neglect syndrome? Cortex. 2004;40:667–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Bäckman L, Jones S, Berger AK, Laukka EJ, Small BJ. Multiple cognitive deficits during the transition to Alzheimer’s disease. J Intern Med. 2004;256(3):195–204. Available from: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01386.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Robertson IH. Cognitive rehabilitation: attention and neglect. Trends Cogn Sci. 1999;3(10):385–93. Available from: https://0-linkinghub-elsevier-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/retrieve/pii/S1364661399013789.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Berlucchi G. Brain plasticity and cognitive neurorehabilitation. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2011;21(5):560–78. Availbale from: http://0-www-tandfonline-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/abs/10.1080/09602011.2011.573255.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Hofgren C, Björkdahl A, Esbjörnsson E, Stibrant-Sunnerhagen K. Recovery after stroke: cognition, ADL function and return to work. Acta Neurol Scand. 2007;115(2):73–80. Available from: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1600-0404.2006.00768.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. De Raedt R, Ponjaert-Kristoffersen I. The relationship between cognitive/neuropsychological factors and Car driving performance in Older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48(12):1664–8 Available from: http://0-doi-wiley-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2000.tb03880.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Maier M, Ballester Rubio B, Duff A, Duarte Oller E, Verschure PFMJ. Effect of specific over nonspecific VR-based rehabilitation on Poststroke motor recovery: a systematic meta-analysis. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2019;33(2):112–29 Available from: https://0-journals-sagepub-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/10.1177/1545968318820169.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Aminov A, Rogers JM, Middleton S, Caeyenberghs K, Wilson PH. What do randomized controlled trials say about virtual rehabilitation in stroke? A systematic literature review and meta-analysis of upper-limb and cognitive outcomes. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2018;15(1):15-29. Available from: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12984-018-0370-2.

  25. Laver KE, Lange B, George S, Deutsch JE, Saposnik G, Crotty M. Virtual reality for stroke rehabilitation. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2017. p. 1465–1858. [cited 2018 Feb 2]. Available from: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/14651858.CD008349.pub4.

  26. de Rooij IJM, van de Port IGL, Meijer J-WG. Effect of Virtual Reality Training on Balance and Gait Ability in Patients With Stroke: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Phys Ther. 2016;96(12):1905–18 [cited 2018 Aug 8]. Available from: https://0-academic-oup-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/ptj/article-lookup/doi/10.2522/ptj.20160054.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Cicerone KD, Langenbahn DM, Braden C, Malec JF, Kalmar K, Fraas M, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the literature from 2003 through 2008. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2011;92(4):519–30 Available from: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.apmr.2010.11.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Maier M, Rubio Ballester B, Verschure PFMJ. Principles of neurorehabilitation after stroke based on motor learning and brain plasticity mechanisms. Front Syst Neurosci. 2019;13:74. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnsys.2019.00074/full.

  29. Pedroli E, Serino S, Cipresso P, Pallavicini F, Riva G. Assessment and rehabilitation of neglect using virtual reality: a systematic review. Front Behav Neurosci. 2015;9(August):1–15 Available from: http://journal.frontiersin.org/Article/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00226/abstract.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Chung C, Pollock A, Campbell T, Durward B, Hagen S. Cognitive Rehabilitation for Executive Dysfunction in Adults With Stroke or Other Adult Nonprogressive Acquired Brain Damage. Stroke. 2013;44(7):e77–8 [cited 2015 Mar 3]. Available from: http://stroke.ahajournals.org/cgi/doi/10.1161/STROKEAHA.113.002049.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Nair R, Cogger H, Worthington E, Lincoln N. Cognitive rehabilitation for memory deficits after stroke (Review). Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2016;9:1465-858. Available from: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/14651858.CD002293.pub3.

  32. Ballester Rubio B, Nirme J, Duarte E, Cuxart A, Rodriguez S, Verschure PFMJ, et al. The visual amplification of goal-oriented movements counteracts acquired non-use in hemiparetic stroke patients. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2015;12(1):50 Available from: http://www.jneuroengrehab.com/content/12/1/50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Mainetti R, Sedda A, Ronchetti M, Bottini G, Borghese NA. Duckneglect: video-games based neglect rehabilitation. Technol Heal Care. 2013;21(2):97–111. Available from: https://www.medra.org/servlet/aliasResolver?alias=iospress&doi=10.3233/THC-120712.

  34. Marteniuk RG. In: Holt R, Holt W, editors. Information processing in motor skills. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston; 1976.

  35. Guadagnoli MA, Lee TD. Challenge point: a framework for conceptualizing the effects of various practice conditions in motor learning. J Mot Behav. 2004;36(2):212–24. Available from: http://0-www-tandfonline-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/abs/10.3200/JMBR.36.2.212-224.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Cicerone KD, Dahlberg C, Kalmar K, Langenbahn DM, Malec JF, Bergquist TF, et al. Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: recommendations for clinical practice. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2000;81(12):1596–615. Available from: https://0-linkinghub-elsevier-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/retrieve/pii/S0003999300105787.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Westerberg H, Jacobaeus H, Hirvikoski T, Clevberger P, Ostensson M-L, Bartfai A, et al. Computerized working memory training after stroke--a pilot study. Brain Inj. 2007;21(1):21–9. Available from: http://0-www-tandfonline-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/full/10.1080/02699050601148726.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Faria AL, Andrade A, Soares L, Bermúdez i Badia S. Benefits of virtual reality based cognitive rehabilitation through simulated activities of daily living: a randomized controlled trial with stroke patients. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2016;13(96):2–12 Available from: http://0-jneuroengrehab-biomedcentral-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/articles/10.1186/s12984-016-0204-z.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Cameirao MS, Bermudez i Badia S, Oller ED, Verschure PFMJ. Using a Multi-Task Adaptive VR System for Upper Limb Rehabilitation in the Acute Phase of Stroke. In: Virtual rehabilitation, vol. 2008: IEEE; 2008. p. 2–7. Available from: https://0-ieeexplore-ieee-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/document/4625112/.

  40. Martens R, Burwitz L, Zuckerman J. Modeling effects on motor performance. Res Quarterly Am Alliance Heal Phys Educ Recreat. 1976;47(2):277–91. Available from: https://0-www-tandfonline-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/full/10.1080/10671315.1976.10615372.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Cameirão MS, Bermúdez i Badia S, Duarte Oller E, Verschure PFMJ. The rehabilitation gaming system: a review. Stud Health Technol Inform. 2009;145(6):65–83.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Verschure PFMJ. Neuroscience, virtual reality and neurorehabilitation: Brain repair as a validation of brain theory. In: 2011 Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society: IEEE; 2011. p. 2254–7. Available from: http://0-ieeexplore-ieee-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/document/6090428/.

  43. Ballester BR, Maier M, Duff A, Cameirão M, Bermúdez S, Duarte E, et al. A critical time window for recovery extends beyond one-year post-stroke. J Neurophysiol. 2019;122(1):350–7. Available from: https://0-www-physiology-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/10.1152/jn.00762.2018.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Ballester BR, Nirme J, Camacho I, Duarte E, Rodríguez S, Cuxart A, et al. Domiciliary VR-Based Therapy for Functional Recovery and Cortical Reorganization: Randomized Controlled Trial in Participants at the Chronic Stage Post Stroke. JMIR Serious Games. 2017;5(3):e15. Available from: http://games.jmir.org/2017/3/e15/.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Prochnow D, Bermúdez i Badia S, Schmidt J, Duff A, Brunheim S, Kleiser R, et al. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of visuomotor processing in a virtual reality-based paradigm: Rehabilitation Gaming System. Eur J Neurosci. 2013;37(9):1441–7 [cited 2014 Nov 26]. Available from: http://0-doi-wiley-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/ejn.12157.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Patel R, Spreng RN, Turner GR. Functional brain changes following cognitive and motor skills training: a quantitative meta-analysis. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2013;27(3):187–99. Available from: http://0-journals-sagepub-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/10.1177/1545968312461718.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Verschure PFMJ, Althaus P. A real-world rational agent: unifying old and new AI. Cogn Sci. 2003;27(4):561–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Wyss R, König P, Verschure PFMJ. Involving the motor system in decision making. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2004;271(SUPPL. 3):50–2. Available from: https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/10.1098/rsbl.2003.0098.

  49. Katz B, Jaeggi S, Buschkuehl M, Stegman A, Shah P. Differential effect of motivational features on training improvements in school-based cognitive training. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8(April):1–10 Available from: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00242/abstract.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Jurkiewicz MT, Marzolini S, Oh P. Adherence to a home-based exercise program for individuals after stroke. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2011;18(3):277–84. Available from: http://0-www-tandfonline-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/full/10.1310/tsr1803-277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Barrett AM, Buxbaum LJ, Coslett HB, Edwards E, Heilman KM, Hillis AE, et al. Cognitive rehabilitation interventions for neglect and related disorders: moving from bench to bedside in stroke patients. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18(7):1223–36 Available from: http://0-www-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov.brum.beds.ac.uk/pubmed/16839294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Piron L, Turolla A, Agostini M, Zucconi C, Cortese F, Zampolini M, et al. Exercises for paretic upper limb after stroke: a combined virtual-reality and telemedicine approach. J Rehabil Med. 2009;41(12):1016–20. Available from: https://medicaljournals.se/jrm/content/abstract/10.2340/16501977-0459.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Srivastava A, Taly A, Gupta A, Murali T. Post-stroke depression: Prevalence and relationship with disability in chronic stroke survivors. Ann Indian Acad Neurol. 2010;13(2):123 Available from: http://www.annalsofian.org/text.asp?2010/13/2/123/64643.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Hackett ML, Pickles K. Part I: Frequency of depression after stroke: an updated systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Int J Stroke. 2014;9(8):1017–25. Available from: http://0-journals-sagepub-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/10.1111/ijs.12357.

  55. Kauhanen ML, Korpelainen JT, Hiltunen P, Brusin E, Mononen H, Maatta R, et al. Poststroke depression correlates with cognitive impairment and neurological deficits. Stroke. 1999;30(9):1875–80 Available from: http://0-www-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov.brum.beds.ac.uk/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=10471439.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. McDermott LM, Ebmeier KP. A meta-analysis of depression severity and cognitive function. J Affect Disord. 2009;119(1–3):1–8 Available from: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.jad.2009.04.022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Kurz A, Pohl C, Ramsenthaler M, Sorg C. Cognitive rehabilitation in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2009;24:163–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Verschure PFMJ. Distributed Adaptive Control: A theory of the Mind, Brain, Body Nexus. Biol Inspired Cogn Archit. 2012;1:55–72 Available from: https://0-linkinghub-elsevier-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/retrieve/pii/S2212683X12000102.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Nirme J, Duff A, Verschure PFMJ. Adaptive rehabilitation gaming system: on-line individualization of stroke rehabilitation. In: 2011 annual international conference of the IEEE engineering in medicine and biology society: IEEE; 2011. p. 6749–52. Available from: http://0-www-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov.brum.beds.ac.uk/pubmed/22255888.

  60. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695–9 Available from: http://0-doi-wiley-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Guarantors of Brain. Aids to the examination of the peripheral nervous system. Ulster Med J. 1989;58:201.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Rist PM, Chalmers J, Arima H, Anderson C, MacMahon S, Woodward M, et al. Baseline cognitive function, recurrent stroke, and risk of dementia in patients with stroke. Stroke. 2013;44(7):1790–5. Available from: https://www.ahajournals.org/doi/10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.680728.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Pendlebury ST, Rothwell PM. Prevalence, incidence, and factors associated with pre-stroke and post-stroke dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8(11):1006–18 Available from: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70236-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Cameirão MS, Badia SB i, Duarte E, Frisoli A, Verschure PFMJ. The Combined Impact of Virtual Reality Neurorehabilitation and Its Interfaces on Upper Extremity Functional Recovery in Patients With Chronic Stroke. Stroke. 2012;43(10):2720–8 [cited 2015 Feb 11]. Available from: https://www.ahajournals.org/doi/10.1161/STROKEAHA.112.653196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Faria AL, Cameirão MS, Couras JF, Aguiar JRO, Costa GM, Bermúdez i Badia S. Combined Cognitive-Motor Rehabilitation in Virtual Reality Improves Motor Outcomes in Chronic Stroke – A Pilot Study. Front Psychol. 2018;9(5):854 Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00854/abstract.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. O’Callaghan CA. OxMaR: open source free software for online minimization and randomization for clinical trials. PLoS One. 2014;9(10):1–10. Available from: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1371/journal.pone.0110761.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  67. da Silva CM, Bermúdez i Badia S, Duarte E, Verschure PFMJ. Virtual reality based rehabilitation speeds up functional recovery of the upper extremities after stroke: a randomized controlled pilot study in the acute phase of stroke using the rehabilitation gaming system. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2011;29(5):287–98 Available from: https://www.medra.org/servlet/aliasResolver?alias=iospress&doi=10.3233/RNN-2011-0599.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Gendolla GHE. Self-relevance of performance, task difficulty, and task engagement assessed as cardiovascular response. Motiv Emot. 1999;23(1):45–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, Marsiske M, et al. Effects of cognitive training interventions with Older adults. Am Med Assoc. 2002;288(18):2271–81 Available from: http://jama.jamanetwork.com/article.aspx?doi=10.1001/jama.288.18.2271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Corsi PM. Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. Diss Abstr Int. 1973;34(2-B):891. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1976-04900-001.

  71. Reitan RM. Validity of the trail making test as an Indicator of organic brain damage. Percept Mot Skills. 1958;8(3):271–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Wechsler D. Wechsler adult intelligence scale (4th ed.). San Antonio: Pearson Assessment; 2008.

  73. Rey A. L’examen psychologique dans les cas d’encéphalopathie traumatique. (Les problems.). Arch Psychol. 1941;28:215–85.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, Pillon B. The FAB: A frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurol Int. 2000;55(11):1621–6 Available from: http://www.embase.com/search/results?subaction=viewrecord&from=export&id=L31000485.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  75. Wilson B, Cockburn J, Halligan P. Development of a behavioral test of visuospatial neglect. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1987;68(2):98–102.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Collin C, Wade DT, Davies S, Horne V. The Barthel ADL index: a reliability study. Int Disabil Stud. 1988;10(2):61–3. Available from: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3109/09638288809164103.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Fugl-Meyer AR, Jääskö L, Leyman I, Olsson S, Steglind S. The post stroke hemiplegic patient. 1. A method for evaluation of physical performance. Scand J Rehabil Med. 1975;7(1):13–31.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;23(56):56–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “MINI-MENTAL STATE” A Practical Method For Grading the Cognitive State of Patients for the Clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–98 Available from: papers2://publication/uuid/C9F59886-73CE-4CFC-AD8F-CAE6B9777609.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Maier M, Banuelos NL, Ballester BR, Duarte E, Verschure PFMJ. Conjunctive rehabilitation of multiple cognitive domains for chronic stroke patients in virtual reality. In: 2017 International Conference on Rehabilitation Robotics (ICORR): IEEE; 2017. p. 947–52. Available from: https://0-ieeexplore-ieee-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/document/8009371/.

  81. Perez-Marcos D, Bieler-Aeschlimann M, Serino A. Virtual reality as a vehicle to empower motor-cognitive Neurorehabilitation. Front Psychol. 2018;9(11):1–8 Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02120/full.

    Google Scholar 

  82. Peña-Casanova J, Quiñones-Úbeda S, Quintana-Aparicio M, Aguilar M, Badenes D, Molinuevo JL, et al. Spanish multicenter normative studies (NEURONORMA project): norms for verbal span, visuospatial span, letter and number sequencing, trail making test, and symbol digit modalities test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2009;24(4):321–41. Available from: https://0-academic-oup-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/acn/article-lookup/doi/10.1093/arclin/acp038.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Vazzana R, Bandinelli S, Lauretani F, Volpato S, Di Iorio A, Abate G, et al. Trail making test predicts physical impairment and mortality in OLDER persons. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2010;58(4):719–23.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Kessels RPC, van den Berg E, Ruis C, Brands AMA. The backward span of the Corsi block-tapping task and its association with the WAIS-III digit span. Assessment. 2008;15(4):426–34 Available from: http://0-www-ncbi-nlm-nih-gov.brum.beds.ac.uk/pubmed/18483192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Lee RSC, Hermens DF, Porter MA, Redoblado-Hodge MA. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in first-episode major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord. 2012;140(2):113–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. van Zandvoort MJE, Kessels RPC, Nys GMS, de Haan EHF, Kappelle LJ. Early neuropsychological evaluation in patients with ischaemic stroke provides valid information. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2005;107(5):385–92. Available from: https://0-linkinghub-elsevier-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/retrieve/pii/S0303846704001659.

  87. Kessels R. P. C., Martine J. E. van Zandvoort, Albert Postma, L. Jaap Kappelle EHF de H. the Corsi block-tapping task: standardization and normative data. Appl Neuropsychol. 2000;7(February 2015):252–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Levin MF, Weiss PL, Keshner EA. Emergence of virtual reality as a tool for upper limb rehabilitation: incorporation of motor control and motor learning principles. Phys Ther. 2015;95(3):415–25 Available from: https://0-academic-oup-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/ptj/article-lookup/doi/10.2522/ptj.20130579.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Cameirão MS, Badia SBI, Oller ED, Verschure PFMJ. Neurorehabilitation using the virtual reality based rehabilitation gaming system: methodology, design, psychometrics, usability and validation. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2010;7(48):48. Available from: http://0-jneuroengrehab-biomedcentral-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/articles/10.1186/1743-0003-7-48.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. McEwen S, Polatajko H, Baum C, Rios J, Cirone D, Doherty M, et al. Combined cognitive-strategy and task-specific training improve transfer to untrained activities in subacute stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2015;29(6):526–36. Available from: http://0-journals-sagepub-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/10.1177/1545968314558602.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  91. Wolinsky FD, Unverzagt FW, Smith DM, Jones R, Stoddard A, Tennstedt SL. The ACTIVE cognitive training trial and health-related quality of life: protection that lasts for 5 years. J Gerontol. 2006;61(12):1324–9. Available from: http://0-academic-oup-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/biomedgerontology/article/61/12/1324/537568/The-ACTIVE-Cognitive-Training-Trial-and.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Dujardin K, Defebvre L, Krystkowiak P, Blond S, Destée A. Influence of chronic bilateral stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus on cognitive function in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol. 2001;248:603–11. Available from: http://0-link-springer-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s004150170139.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Desmond DW, Moroney JT, Sano M, Stern Y. Recovery of cognitive function after stroke. Stroke. 1996;27(10):1798–803. Available from: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3543398&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Pendlebury ST, Mariz J, Bull L, Mehta Z, Rothwell PM. MoCA, ACE-R, and MMSE versus the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke–Canadian Stroke network vascular cognitive impairment harmonization standards neuropsychological battery after TIA and Stroke. Stroke. 2012;43(2):464–9. Available from: https://www.ahajournals.org/doi/10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.633586.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Page SJ, Fulk GD, Boyne P. Clinically Important Differences for the Upper-Extremity Fugl-Meyer Scale in People With Minimal to Moderate Impairment Due to Chronic Stroke. Phys Ther. 2012;92(6):791–8 [cited 2018 Mar 13]. Available from: https://0-academic-oup-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/ptj/article/92/6/791/2735128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Hsueh IP, Hsu MJ, Sheu CF, Lee S, Hsieh CL, Lin JH. Psychometric comparisons of 2 versions of the Fugl-Meyer Motor scale and 2 versions of the stroke rehabilitation assessment of movement. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2008;22(6):737–44. Available from: http://0-journals-sagepub-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/10.1177/1545968308315999.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Gonzalez MT, Hartig T, Patil GG, Martinsen EW, Kirkevold M. Therapeutic horticulture in clinical depression: a prospective study. Res Theory Nurs Pract. 2009;23(4):312–28. Available from: http://0-connect-springerpub-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/lookup/doi/10.1891/1541-6577.23.4.312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Borsboom D, Cramer AOJ. Network analysis: an integrative approach to the structure of psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2013;9(1):91–121. Available from: http://0-www-annualreviews-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/doi/abs/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185608.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all the patients that participated in and contributed to the study. A special thanks go to Nohora Rueda and Susana Redon, occupational therapists at Hospital de l’Esperança, for their assistance and support during the experiments.

Funding

This study was supported by EIT Health ID 19277 (RGS@HOME) under H2020, SANAR (MINECO, TIN2013–44200), cDAC (ERC-2013-ADG-341196), and socSMCs (641321H2020-FETPROACT-2014).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors made a substantial contribution to the conception of the trial. Technical implementation, analysis of the data, and drafting of the manuscript were performed by MM. Data collection was performed by MM and NLB. All authors were involved in the interpretation of the data and revising the manuscript. All authors provided approval of the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul F. M. J. Verschure.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This randomized controlled pilot trial was approved by the local Ethical Committee at Parc de Salut Mar, and all eligible patients enrolled in the study gave their written consent to participate. No patient without consent participated in this trial.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare the following potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: PFMJV is the founder and interim CEO of Eodyne S L, which aims at bringing scientifically validated neurorehabilitation technology to society. The rest of the authors have nothing to disclose.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1. Supplementary Material.

The document contains further information on the experimental intervention and the statistical procedure as well as additional Figures S1 and S2 and additional Tables S1 to S5 not shown but referenced in the main manuscript.

Additional file 2:

Movie S1. A video showing the intervention in action.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Maier, M., Ballester, B.R., Leiva Bañuelos, N. et al. Adaptive conjunctive cognitive training (ACCT) in virtual reality for chronic stroke patients: a randomized controlled pilot trial. J NeuroEngineering Rehabil 17, 42 (2020). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12984-020-0652-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12984-020-0652-3

Keywords