Skip to main content

Altered regional homogeneity in experimentally induced low back pain: a resting-state fMRI study

Abstract

Background

Functional imaging studies have indicated that patients with low back pain can have significant reductions in cerebral cortex grey matter. However, the mechanisms governing the nociceptive pathways in the human brain are unclear. The aim of this study was to use functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and regional homogeneity (ReHo) to investigate changes in resting-state brain activity in subjects that experienced experimentally induced low back pain.

Methods

Healthy subjects (n = 15) underwent fMRI (3.0 T) at baseline and during painful stimulation (intramuscular injection of 3% hypertonic saline).

Results

Compared to the scans conducted at baseline, scans conducted during experimentally induced low back pain showed increased ReHo on the right side in the medial prefrontal cortex, precuneus, insula, parahippocampal gyrus and cerebellum (posterior lobe), but decreased ReHo in the primary somatosensory cortex, anterior cingulate cortex and parahippocampal gyrus on the left side. The right inferior parietal lobule also showed a decreased ReHo (P < 0.05, cluster threshold ≥10).

Conclusions

These findings suggest that abnormally spontaneous resting-state activity in some brain regions may be associated with pain processing. These changes in neural activity may contribute to the recognition, execution, memory and emotional processing of acute low back pain.

Introduction

Pain is processed in multiple brain areas, thus indicating the complexity of pain perception. Chronic low back pain (LBP) is increasingly viewed as a condition that affects normal brain function beyond the neural activity related to pain perception, leads to mental health problems, including depression, anxiety and sleeping disturbances [1, 2]. Irrespective of the location, nature or course of the different pain syndromes, our understanding of the neural mechanisms of LBP has progressed through investigations examining the structural changes, relating marked reductions of grey matter in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and insular cortices [3, 4]. These studies have shown that grey matter changes are correlated with not only cortical activity but also the duration,and intensity of pain [5], suggesting that the structural changes in the brain result from functional changes. In the past decade, neuroimaging techniques have been used to identify functional changes that are important for cortical processing of pain. Individuals experiencing pain show abnormal neural activity in response to nociceptive stimuli [1, 68]. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies examining chronic LBP have indicated that the temporal and spatial properties of functional connectivity are disrupted when pain is perceived [2, 9, 10]. Moreover, chronic LBP can induce negative affective responses, such as unpleasantness, suffering, and other negative effects, which induce a pronounced activation of the pain matrix [11].

Although changes in neural activity related to chronic LBP have been extensively studied, little is known about how a network of selective painful stimuli is spatially represented in acute condition. Acute pain is typically accompanied by brief intervals of unpleasant emotions, anxiety, fear or avoidance behaviour. We suspect that brain activation patterns differ between individuals with chronic pain and acute pain. Considering acute LBP symptoms are paroxysmal and it is difficult for subjects to endure painful stimuli during fMRI scanning that can last more than 5 min, a proper pain model should be established in healthy subjects to improve the understanding of the neural mechanisms related underlying acute LBP. Intramuscular hypertonic saline injection is considered as a valid experimental model of acute pain in humans, and has been used in studies investigating orofacial muscle pain [12], dynamic neural activation [13] and discrete changes in cortical activity [14]. The advantage of this method is that it directly stimulates the muscle, thus causing persistent nociceptive stimulation that would enable a mechanistic study of brain activity during pain perception. Investigating brain activity during acute LBP would also help to explore the potential mechanisms through which acute pain can progress to chronic pain.

Neuroimaging can identify subtle changes in brain networks, and therefore can detect and localise nociceptive responses in the brain [15, 16]. Resting-state fMRI has revealed that spontaneous neural activity is highly correlated with low frequency (<0.08 Hz) blood-oxygen-level-dependent signals [17, 18]. Other studies suggest that brain fMRI can reveal the biochemical, structural and functional information that reflects physiological integrity in vivo [19]. Basic sensory and motor networks as well as several higher-order resting-state networks (RSNs) that primarily contain interconnections between heteromodal associated cortical regions can be identified by fMRI [20]. The default mode network (DMN), one of several RSNs, is the most thoroughly investigated and stable RSNs that provides ‘a balance of opposing forces’ and enhances ‘the maintenance of information for interpreting, responding to and even predicting environmental demands’ [18]. Acute muscle pain can alter brain dynamics, particularly responses of the DMN [14, 21, 22]. Moreover, somatotopic reorganisation occurs in the primary and secondary sensory areas in response to both chronic and experimentally induced acute pain [2325].

Here, we present a novel method based on regional homogeneity (ReHo) to analyse spontaneous resting-state brain activity. The ReHo quantifies the similarities among several time series of fMRI signals and takes into account the resting-state activity across the entire brain. Compared to classic model-driven methods, ReHo appears more sensitive to spontaneous haemodynamic responses and can detect unpredictable haemodynamic responses that model-driven methods fail to identify. Because ReHo represents the synchronised neural activity in a specific region, it can serve as a complementary tool for investigating resting brain activity and regional stability [26]. Recent studies have demonstrated that abnormal ReHo can occur in migraine [27], Alzheimer’s disease [28] and Parkinson’s disease [29]. These results confirm the diagnostic validity of ReHo for neuropsychiatric disorders. However, it remains unclear whether subjects with acute LBP demonstrate changes in regional coherence in response to noxious stimuli.

Based on previous evidence showing extensively altered brain dynamics in chronic low back pain patients [2, 8], we report the generality of the results presented by Apkarian [1] by determining whether dynamic changes in RSN with spontaneous brain activity occur with tonic muscle pain. We hypothesised that changes in ReHo measurements of brain activity should be evident in experimentally induced acute LBP. The expected altered brain processing should involve mainly the brain structures mediating the recognition and emotional dimensions of pain, including the DMN, insula and hippocampus cortex.

Materials and methods

Participating subjects

Healthy subjects (n = 15) similar in age (22–30 years old, mean = 25.7), education (14–21 years, mean = 17) and approximately equal numbers’ gender (eight males and seven females) participated in this study. All subjects were right-handed and all gave informed consent to procedures approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of Zhujiang Hospital Affiliated to Southern Medical University (Guangzhou). Inclusion criteria included: (1) Body mass index (BMI) with a range of ± 10% from standard BMI (Mainland, China); (2) No neurological or psychiatric illnesses (i.e. multiple sclerosis, epilepsy); (3) No pain (including dysmenorrhea) or drug use (i.e. antipyretics, sleeping pills) within the last month; (4) Self-rating anxiety scale (SAS) [30] and self-rating depression scale (SDS) [31] scores < 50.

Stimulus and data acquisitions

With subjects in a prone position, a fine plastic cannula (24 gauge) attached to a 1 ml syringe containing sterile hypertonic (3%) saline was inserted 2 cm deep into the right back muscle, at the level of lumbar vertebra 4, prior to scanning for 10 min. The pain that caused by the needle subsided to pre-injection levels within approximately 20 s under non-anesthesia condition. A 3 T Philips Achieva whole-body scanner with single shot gradient echo-planar imaging (GRE-EPI) capability was used to conduct fMRI scans using a standard radio-frequency head coil. Multislice T2*-weighted scans were obtained using the following parameters: repetition time (TR) = 3 s; echo time (TE) = 40 ms; flip angle = 90°; field of view (FOV) = 220 mm × 220 mm; matrix = 64 × 64; 3.54 mm × 3.54 mm in-plane resolution; slice thickness, 5 mm; slice gap, 0.5 mm; 24 axial slices. Each subject underwent two identical fMRI at health status as baseline and after hypertonic saline as the pain stimulus. Following a 318 s baseline, all 15 subjects received an intramuscular injection of 3% hypertonic saline (0.3 ml) and the same fMRI scans were repeated 20 s after the injection. A high resolution T1-weighted fast spin echo anatomical scan was also acquired with TR/TE = 500/14 ms; matrix = 256 × 256; and FOV of 220 mm before baseline. Subjects were asked to remain still in the scanner and to keep their mind blank, eyes closed and avoid falling asleep. While still inside the scanner after scans, subjects were asked to provide verbal responses on their pain intensity using a visual analogue scale (VAS) [32], and a homemade mood scale for pain unpleasantness (including distressing, horrible). Moreover, subjects were required to rate the maximal pain intensity (0 ~ 10, 0 = “no pain” and 10 = “worst pain imaginable”) and unpleasantness (0 ~ 10, 0 = “Minimum” and 10 = “Maximal”), which evaluated the difference among the test before, on and after, as well as describe the time of onset, peak and cessation of pain. The locations of significantly activated clusters in the brain were determined by using standard anatomical landmarks (e.g. the postcentral gyrus is defined as S1).

ReHo analysis

The BOLD signal was preprocessed using Data Processing Assistant for Resting-State fMRI (DPARSF, http://www.restfmri.net/forum/DPARSF) including image conversion, removal of the first ten volumes, slice-timing correction, motion correction, intensity normalization, detrending and filtering. All subjects’ head movements were less than 2 mm in translation and 2° in rotation by obtaining motion time courses (no subjects were excluded). Each individual’s functional images were normalized to the standard EPI templates and resampled with 3 × 3 × 3 mm3 resolution. A linear high-pass consisting of edge and high frequency artifact removal by linear regression was performed. A zero lag finite impulse response filter was applied to the band pass filter (0.01 Hz < f < 0.08Hz). The output data was then processed to produce ReHo map image files after filtering. The ReHo analysis was based on previous reports [26] and calculated as Kendall’s coefficient to measure the ReHo or similarity of ranked time series of a given voxel with its nearest 26 neighboring voxels in a voxel-wise way. The Kendall’s coefficient value was calculated to this voxel and an individual Kendall’s coefficient map was obtained for each subject. Each ReHo map was divided by its own mean ReHo within the mask for standardization purposes [26]. Spatial smoothing was conducted using a Gaussian kernel of full-width-half-maximum 6 mm to reduce the noise and residual differences in the gyral anatomy.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 13.0 software. Descriptive statistics (mean and standard deviation) were calculated for age, gender, education, pain intensity and pain unpleasantness. A one-way repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to compare the unpleasantness ratings before, during and after the MRI scans. Second level random effect analyses were used to create within group Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) and Xjview (http://www.alivelearn.net/xjview) for each network and to explore the ReHo differences between the two conditions. One-sample t-tests were performed for each network that had a threshold at the whole-brain cluster-level for P < 0.05 after correction for multiple comparisons using the false discovery rate (FDR) and a minimum cluster size of 10 voxels to extract the ReHo results across the subjects within each condition. To compare ReHo changes of the brain, two-tailed paired t-tests were used to compare group’s average percentage signal-intensity changes that threshold at the whole-brain cluster-level corrected at a significance level of 0.05 for multiple comparisons (FDR) using a minimum cluster size of 10 vcxels.

Results

All patients were awake throughout the fMRI scans, rated the pain intensity as 3 or higher and did not produce head movements greater than 2 mm in translation or 2° in rotation. Therefore, data for all patients was included in the study.

Pain perception

Unilateral (right) injections of hypertonic saline into the back muscle caused pain that began within 15 s of the injection and subsided to pre-injection levels within 10 min of the injection. The muscle pain was described as dull and aching with mild numbness. The average maximum pain score following the intramuscular injection was 6.33 ± 2.29 (range, 3–9). As the muscle pain was induced, subjects described pain that rostrally progressed and encompassed a region surrounding the injection site with a 2–3 cm diameter. During the test, ‘distressing’ (2.92 ± 1.78) and ‘horrible’ (1.50 ± 1.62) scores, which indicated that subjects experienced mild distress or dread. However, the differences of pain unpleasantness before, during or after the MRI scans were not statistically significant (ANOVA, P > 0.05).

fMRI ReHo alterations

The ReHo maps for pain status and baseline were consistent (Figure 1), but there were significant differences in brain activity (P < 0.05, FDR, cluster threshold ≥10). Second level analysis revealed several differences among the frontal lobe, temporal lobe, parietal lobe and cingulate cortex. Increased pain status scores corresponded to increased ReHo in the right mPFC, right middle frontal gyrus, right insula, right precuneus, right parahippocampal gyrus and right cerebellum (posterior lobe-cerebellar tonsil; Table 1). In contrast, the pain status scores corresponded to significant decreases in ReHo values in the right superior temporal gyrus, left middle temporal gyrus, left primary somatosensory cortex (S1), left ACC, left parahippocampal gyrus and right inferior parietal lobule (IPL; Figure 2). These changes suggest the spatial topography of the brain was altered with experimentally induced LBP.

Figure 1
figure 1

Resting-state regional homogeneity (ReHo) of one-sample t -test in baseline (A) and pain status (B) ( P< 0.05, cluster threshold ≥ 10). Based on the normal topography of the selected resting-state networks, back muscle pain evoked ReHo abnormalities in the frontal lobe, parietal lobule, cingulate cortex and cerebellum. Note: Regional positive in ReHo for each network are shown as red, whereas negative are shown in black.

Table 1 Resting-state regional homogeneity alterations corresponding to pain status
Figure 2
figure 2

Resting-state regional homogeneity (ReHo) of paired t -test between baseline and pain status ( P< 0.05, cluster threshold ≥ 10). Compared to baseline, acute low back pain demonstrated higher ReHo in the right medial prefrontal cortex, right middle frontal gyrus, right insula, right precuneus, right parahippocampal gyrus and right cerebellar tonsil. In contrast, pain status demonstrated lower ReHo in the right superior temporal gyrus, left middle temporal gyrus, left primary somatosensory cortex (S1), left anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), left parahippocampal gyrus and right inferior parietal lobule. Note: Status-averaged increases are red–yellow and decreases are blue–green.

Discussion

In the present experiment, we used the ReHo method to investigate changes in experimentally induced LBP and examined pain processing among several brain regions. This investigation was motivated by hypothesis that persistent pain can induce changes in the brain beyond those that take place in the cortical circuits typically involved in pain processing. Our findings confirm this hypothesis and demonstrate that acute LBP corresponds to severe ReHo changes in several brain regions. Thus, using back pain to infer relevant information about pain processing in the brain with LBP is fully justified. Our approach was prolific and revealed that acute LBP affects normal brain function in the resting state.

Back pain evoked abnormal ReHo in the DMN. The DMN is characterised by balanced positive and negative correlations between the activity of the mPFC, PCC/precuneus, IPL and mesial temporal lobe structures. Furthermore, this activity correlates with behaviour, emotional measures and self-referential processes [3336]. Consistent with previous findings where acute painful stimuli disrupted DMN activity [14, 21, 22], the present findings show abnormally spontaneous resting-state activity in DMN in acute LBP. The mPFC and precuneus exhibit significantly higher ReHo values than other brain regions. The mPFC is associated with processing emotional information and mediating functional interactions among brain regions that participate in pain processing [36]; the precuneus is likely involved in shifting attention between different spatial locations [37]. Therefore, changes in ReHo may reflect pain that is accompanied by the processing of emotionally intense information. Moreover, increased pain levels corresponded to decreased ReHo in the middle temporal gyrus. This may reflect abnormalities in experience-dependent maturation of cortical functional specialisation, and could be the consequence of increased inhibitory drive or asynchronous neuronal firing.

Some imaging studies with humans reported that muscle pain evoked signal increases in the mPFC, insula, S1 and cerebellar cortices [22, 38, 39]. Our study showed that pain perceived in back muscle evoked higher ReHo values in the mPFC, insula and cerebellum that were accompanied by lower values in the left S1 and right IPL. Considering the lateral pathway projects to S1, this cortical area is considered to encode the sensory-discriminative aspects of pain, such as location, intensity and quality [40, 41]. The difference in results between the present and previous investigations may have resulted from differences in how noxious stimuli were delivered. We used stimuli that evoked pain in areas that were relatively larger than those in previous studies that delivered stimuli with heat thermodes or lasers. Furthermore, the changes in ReHo for S1 and contralateral IPL may be related to the earlier observation of somatotopic changes associated with pain intensity, and explain the sensory disturbances common to LBP.

A previous study reported an increased ReHo in the left ACC with an increased chronic back pain [10]. This is inconsistent with our present finding that ReHo decreased with acute LBP that prompted different ACC activation patterns between individuals with persistent pain and healthy subjects [42]. ACC participates in pain perception and integrating the sensory, attentional and cognitive components of pain [43, 44]. The ReHo decreases in ACC suggests reductions in efficient pain processing or compensatory damage in functionally relevant regions, such as the prefrontal cortex and caudate [45]. It is well documented that pain may interrupt cognition and sustained attention to direct action toward a painful stimulus or threat [19]. The data further support the idea that cingulotomy alters pain perception so that pain no longer interferes with an individual’s daily activities [46].

Interestingly, we observed that activity evoked with acute painful stimulation was significantly different among brain regions. ReHo values particularly decreased in the left parahippocampal gyrus but increased in the right parahippocampal gyrus, as compared with the ‘no pain’ condition. The parahippocampal cortex provides the major poly-sensory input to the hippocampus and receives many combinations of sensory information [47]. In particular, the right parahippocampal gyrus is more involved in detecting and recognising pain than just perceiving pain [48]. The opposite results during acute muscle pain stimulation support the idea that left and right parahippocampal regions play different roles in attenuating pain perception. Therefore, abnormal ReHo results may represent the aberrant conditioning to sensory stimuli that occurs in LBP.

Our findings consequently not only indicate that regional resting-state abnormalities are associated with functional modulation during pain processing but also that changes evoked with acute LBP differ from those evoked with chronic LBP. Future studies using larger sample sizes with a sufficient number of subjects for performing meaningful comparisons between different gender disorders are needed. Furthermore, abundant phenotypic data is needed to help confirm these preliminary findings and to fully characterise changes in RSNs that process LBP.

Conclusions

We demonstrated that experimentally induced LBP is associated with substantial spatial changes in regional homogeneity for brain regions active in the resting state. The disruptions of ReHo observed in the present study may be related to earlier observations of spontaneous brain activity occurring with persistent pain. We speculate that changes in ReHo are associated with the structural and functional plasticity of pain processing. These changes may account for the recognition, execution, emotional and memory processes involved in acute LBP. We hope that our findings will provide information that can help improve diagnostic and treatment strategies for patients with LBP.

References

  1. Apkarian AV, Bushnell MC, Treede RD, Zubieta JK: Human brain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease. Eur J Pain 2005, 9: 463-484.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Baliki MN, Geha PY, Apkarian AV, Chialvo DR: Beyond feeling: chronic pain hurts the brain, disrupting the default-mode network dynamics. J Neurosci 2008, 28: 1398-1403.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. May A: Neuroimaging: visualising the brain in pain. Neurol Sci 2007, 28: 101-107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Apkarian AV, Sosa Y, Sonty S, Levy RE, Harden R, Parrish T, Gitelman D: Chronic back pain is associated with decreased prefrontal and thalamic gray matter density. J Neurosci 2004, 24: 10410-10415.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. DaSilva AF, Becerra L, Pendse G, Chizh B, Tully S, Borsook D: Colocalized structural and functional changes in the cortex of patients with trigeminal neuropathic pain. PLoS One 2008, 3: e3396.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Cauda F, D’Agata F, Sacco K, Duca S, Cocito D, Paolasso I, Isoardo G, Geminiani G: Altered resting state attentional networks in diabetic neuropathic pain. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2010, 81: 806-811.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Bolwerk A, Seifert F, Maihöfne C: Altered resting-state functional connectivity in complex regional pain syndrome. J Pain 2013, 14: 1107-1115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Napadow V, Kim J, Clauw DJ, Harris RE: Decreased intrinsic brain connectivity is associated with reduced clinical pain in fibromyalgia. Arthritis Rheum 2012, doi: 1–14.

  9. Tagliazucchi E, Balenzuela P, Fraiman D, Chialvo D: Brain resting state is disrupted in chronic back pain patients. Neurosci Lett 2010, 485: 26-31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Baliki MN, Chialvo DR, Geha PY, Levy RM, Harden RN, Parrish TB, Apkarian AV: Chronic pain and the emotional brain: specific brain activity associated with spontaneous fluctuations of intensity of chronic back pain. J Neurosci 2006, 26: 12165-12173.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Witting N, Kupers RC, Svensson P, Jensen TS: A PET activation study of brush-evoked allodynia in patients with nerve injury pain. Pain 2006, 120: 145-154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Nash PG, Macefield VG, Klineberg IJ, Gustin SM, Murray FM, Henderson LA: Bilateral activation of the trigeminothalamic tract by acute orofacial cutaneous and muscle pain in humans. Pain 2010, 151: 384-393.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Owen DG, Clarke CF, Ganapathy S, Prato FS, St Lawrence KS: Using perfusion MRI to measure the dynamic changes in neural activation associated with tonic muscular pain. Pain 2010, 148: 375-386.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Macefield VG, Gandevia SC, Henderson LA: Discrete changes in cortical activation during experimentally induced referred muscle pain: a single-trial fMRI study. Cereb Cortex 2007, 17: 2050-2059.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Fox MD, Raichle ME: Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosc 2007, 8: 700-711.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME: The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2005, 102: 9673-9678.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Raichle ME: The brain’s dark energy. Science 2006, 314: 1249-1250.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL: A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2001, 98: 676-682.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Chang C, Shyu BC: A fMRI study of brain activations during non-noxious and noxious electrical stimulation of the sciatic nerve of rats. Brain Res 2001, 897: 71-81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V: Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2003, 100: 253-258.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Henderson LA, Bandler R, Gandevia SC, Macefield VG: Distinct forebrain activity patterns during deep versus superficial pain. Pain 2006, 120: 286-296.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Bornhovd K, Quante M, Glauche V, Bromm B, Weiller C, Buchel C: Painful stimuli evoke different stimulus–response functions in the amygdala, prefrontal, insula and somatosensory cortex: a single-trial fMRI study. Brain 2002, 125: 1326-1336.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Flor H, Elbert T, Knecht S, Wienbruch C, Pantev C, Birbaumers N, Larbig W, Taub E: Phantom-limb pain as a perceptual correlate of cortical reorganization following arm amputation. Nature 1995, 375: 482-484.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Maih€ofner C, Handwerker HO, Neund€orfer B, Birklein F: Patterns of cortical reorganization in complex regional pain syndrome. Neurology 2003, 61: 1707-1715.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. S€or€os P, Knecht S, Bantel C, Imai T, W€usten R, Pantev C, L€utkenh€oner B, B€urkle H, Henningsen H: Functional reorganization of the human primary somatosensory cortex after acute pain demonstrated by magnetoencephalography. Neurosci Lett 2001, 298: 195-198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Zang Y, Jiang T, Lu Y, He Y, Tian L: Regional homogeneity approach to fMRI data analysis. Neuroimage 2004, 22: 394-400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Yu DH, Yuan K, Zhao L, Zhao LM, Dong MH, Liu P, Wang GH, Liu JX, Sun JB, Zhou GY, Von Deneen KM, Liang FR, Qin W, Tian J: Regional homogeneity abnormalities in patients with interictal migraine without aura: a resting-state study. NMR Biomed 2012, 25: 805-812.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. He Y, Wang L, Zang Y, Tian L, Zhang X, Li K, Jiang T: Regional coherence changes in the early stages of Alzheimer’s disease: a combined structural and resting-state functional MRI study. Neuroimage 2007, 35: 488-500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Wu T, Long X, Zang Y, Wang L, Hallett M, Li K, Chan P: Regional homogeneity changes in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Hum Brain Mapp 2009, 30: 1502-1510.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Zung WW: A self-rating depression scale. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1965, 12: 63-70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Zung WW: A rating instrument for anxiety disorders. Psychosomatics 1971, 12: 371-379.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Kanda M, Matsuhashi M, Sawamoto N, Oga T, Mima T, Nagamine T, Shibasaki H: Cortical potentials related to assessment of pain intensity with visual analogue scale (VAS). Clin Neurophysiol 2002, 113: 1013-1024.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, Reiss AL, Greicius MD: Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci 2007, 27: 2349-2356.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Buckner RL, Vincent JL: Unrest at rest: default activity and spontaneous network correlations. Neuroimage 2007, 37: 1091-1096.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Otti A, G€undel H, Wohlschl€ager A, Zimmer C, Sorg C, Noll-Hussong M: Default mode network of the brain-Neurobiology and clinical significance. Nervenarzt 2011, 83: 16-24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Bantick SJ, Wise RG, Ploghaus A, Clare S, Smith SM, Tracey I: Imaging how attention modulates pain in humans using functional MRI. Brain 2002, 125: 310-319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Wenderoth N, Debaere F, Sunaert S, Swinnen SP: The role of anterior cingulated cortex and precuneus in the coordination of motor behaviour. Eur J Neurosci 2005, 22: 235-246.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Derbyshire SW, Jones AK: Cerebral responses to a continual tonic pain stimulus measured using positron emission tomography. Pain 1988, 76: 127-135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Bingel U, Lorenz J, Glauche V, Knab R, Glascher J, Weiller C, Buchel C: Somatotopic organization of human somatosensory cortices for pain: a single trial fMRI study. Neuroimage 2004, 23: 224-232.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Peyron R, Laurent B, Garcia-Larrea L: Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis. Neurophysiol Clin 2000, 30: 263-288.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Kenshalo DR Jr, Chudler EH, Anton F, Dubner R: SI nociceptive neurons participate in the encoding process by which monkeys perceive the intensity of noxious thermal stimulation. Brain Res 1988, 454: 378-382.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Buffington ALH, Hanlon CA, McKeown MJ: Acute and persistent pain modulation of attention-related anterior cingulate fMRI activations. Pain 2005, 113: 174-184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Fulbright RK, Troche CJ, Skudlarski P, Gore JC, Wexler BE: Functional MR imaging of regional brain activation associated with the affective experience of pain. AJR Am J Roentgenol 2001, 177: 1205-1210.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Hutchison WD, Davis KD, Lozano AM, Tasker RR, Dostrovsky JO: Pain-related neurons in the human cingulate cortex. Nat Neurosci 1999, 2: 403-405.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Oshiro Y, Quevedo AS, McHaffie JG, Kraft RA, Coghill RC: Brain mechanisms supporting discrimination of sensory features of pain: a new model. J Neurosci 2009, 29: 14924-14931.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Cohen RA, Kaplan RF, Zuffante P, Moser DJ, Jenkins MA, Salloway S, Wilkinson H: Alteration of intention and self-initiated action associated with bilateral anterior cingulotomy. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1999, 11: 444-453.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Burwell RD: The parahippocampal region: corticocortical connectivity. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2000, 911: 25-42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Luck D, Danion JM, Marrer C, Pham BT, Gounot D, Foucher J: The right parahippocampal gyrus contributes to the formation and maintenance of bound information in working memory. Brain Cogn 2010, 72: 255-263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the Science and Technology Foundation of Guangdong Province, China (2012B031800305). We thank all volunteers for the assistance in the scanning.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Wen Wu or Guo-zhi Huang.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

SSZ designed and conducted the experiment, performed the analysis and wrote the manuscript. WW designed and conducted the experiment and was involved in revising the manuscript. ZPL conducted the experiment and performed the analysis. GZH made contribution to conception and design, interpretation of data, and revising the manuscript. JMY contributed to conception and design of the study. SGG conducted the experiment and made contribution for the interpretation of data. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Wen Wu, Guo-zhi Huang contributed equally to this work.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhang, Ss., Wu, W., Liu, Zp. et al. Altered regional homogeneity in experimentally induced low back pain: a resting-state fMRI study. J NeuroEngineering Rehabil 11, 115 (2014). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1743-0003-11-115

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1743-0003-11-115

Keywords